Abstract
Differences in the correlated activity of networked brain regions have been reported in individuals with generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) but an overreliance on null-hypothesis significance testing (NHST) limits the identification of disorder-relevant relationships. In this preregistered study, we applied both a Bayesian statistical framework and NHST to the analysis of resting-state fMRI scans from females with GAD and matched healthy comparison females. Eleven a-priori hypotheses about functional connectivity (FC) were evaluated using Bayesian (multilevel model) and frequentist (t-test) inference. Reduced FC between the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) and the posterior-mid insula (PMI) was confirmed by both statistical approaches and was associated with anxiety sensitivity. FC between the vmPFC-anterior insula, the amygdala-PMI, and the amygdala-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) region pairs did not survive multiple comparison correction using the frequentist approach. However, the Bayesian model provided evidence for these region pairs having decreased FC in the GAD group. Leveraging Bayesian modeling, we demonstrate decreased FC of the vmPFC, insula, amygdala, and dlPFC in females with GAD. Exploiting the Bayesian framework revealed FC abnormalities between region pairs excluded by the frequentist analysis and other previously undescribed regions in GAD, demonstrating the value of applying this approach to resting-state FC data in clinical investigations.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) is a psychiatric disorder characterized by disproportionate and uncontrollable worry in addition to somatic symptoms including muscle tension, sleep disturbances, fatigue, and difficulty concentrating. It is a common anxiety disorder, and is associated with substantial functional impairments and economic costs as well as high rates of comorbidity with other psychiatric disorders1. While the neurobiology of GAD has been investigated extensively2, technical advancements in functional neuroimaging in recent decades have afforded insights into abnormalities of regional and network-level neural communication underlying this condition3. Results from many imaging studies suggest that brain regions are organized in distinguishable networks that facilitate complex cognitive functions4. Given the aforementioned functional impairments in GAD it is conceivable that these networks (or the nodes within them) are dysfunctional as well5,6. Among the most frequently described neural networks are the default mode network (DMN, active during the absence of a specific task)7, the salience network (SN, responsible for shifting attention to behaviorally relevant internal and external stimuli)8, and the central executive network (CEN, involved in cognitively demanding functions like management of attention)9. Although only a few studies have examined these three networks explicitly in GAD and with heterogenous results10,11,12, the respective brain regions associated with these networks have been investigated both during task-experiments and during the resting state (for reviews see5,6).
The most common technique for evaluating neural communication at the human network level is functional connectivity (FC) analysis, which involves assessing temporally dependent co-activation of anatomically separated brain regions4. Extant studies on FC in GAD have suggested abnormal relationships between specific brain regions, including the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC)13, the insular cortex6,14, the amygdala5,6,13,15,16, and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC)13. Additionally, analyses of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data suggest that GAD is characterized by abnormal local responses in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC)17 and the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC)18 in task-based experiments and by altered FC of the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC)12 and the temporal pole (TP)16 at rest. The aforementioned brain regions have been associated with numerous mental processes relevant to the psychopathology of GAD (described in Supplement), and many of these regions are key components of the DM, SN, and CEN.
To date, most studies on resting state FC in GAD have selectively interrogated relationships between subsets of brain regions, often relying purely on the null-hypothesis significance testing (NHST) framework12,13,14,15,16,17. While this form of frequentist inference requires several assumptions, one of them is particularly challenging in the context of neuroimaging: the conventional mass-univariate analysis unrealistically assumes uniform distribution across spatial units (i.e., voxels, regions). As effects across the brain tend to approximately follow a normal distribution, the conventional approach suffers from issues such as information loss, overfitting, and artificial dichotomization19. Further, parameter estimation in NHST stabilizes over large sample sizes20, but these samples are not readily obtainable in clinical populations. Even though more robust, non-parametric methods (e.g., threshold-free cluster enhancement21) have been developed within the frequentist framework, inherent limitations of NHST suggest the need for additional, if not alternative, ways of looking at the data.
Bayesian inference is an approach able to assess evidence in the data both for and against the experimental hypotheses, by allowing the researcher to assess for evidence of invariances as well as differences in a variable of interest22. In addition, instead of treating each spatial unit as an isolated entity, as in the conventional mass-univariate NHST analysis, Bayesian multilevel modeling integrates all spatial units into one holistic framework in which all the information is shared and leveraged through partial pooling19. The recent implementation of the multilevel Bayesian modeling matrix-based analysis program (MBA) in AFNI23 is one such example, which enables researchers to infer the probability of a research hypothesis, given the data, while overcoming the issue of multiplicity24.
In this preregistered study, we applied a Bayesian statistical framework to the analysis of resting state FC in GAD, with the addition of a frequentist analysis for a conventional comparison. We assessed the FC of brain regions previously implicated during task-experiments and during the resting state in GAD (vmPFC, dmPFC, dlPFC, dACC, insula, amygdala, PCC, TP) with respect to a focused set of hypotheses regarding potential group differences relative to healthy comparisons (HC) (Table 1). In addition to testing hypotheses stemming from the prior frequentist literature on GAD, the application of multilevel Bayesian modeling enabled us to more effectively address certain issues associated with NHST such as the problem of multiplicity and to evaluate observed relationships for convergence (i.e., to functionally “dissect” the data) across analytic approaches.
Methods
The study hypotheses and data analysis plan were registered on the Open Science Framework before any of the study data was accessed or processed and all study data and analysis scripts are available online25.
Participants
The study sample consisted of 58 participants (nGAD = 29, nHC = 29) matched on measured BMI and self-reported age. Three participants were excluded from further analysis due to excessive motion or signal outliers during their resting scan (see “Preprocessing” section), resulting in a final analysis sample of 27 GAD and 28 HC participants (Fig. S1). The diagnosis of GAD was verified by an experienced clinician administering the MINI neuropsychiatric interview26 according to the DSM-5 criteria27 of excessive anxiety occurring more days than not for at least 6 months, difficulty controlling the worry, consequent impairment in important areas of functioning not attributable to substance effects or other medical conditions, and three of the six key symptoms: restlessness, being easily fatigued, difficulty concentrating, irritability, muscle tension, or sleep disturbance. Additional GAD inclusion criteria were a currently elevated level of anxiety as evidenced by a GAD-7 questionnaire score greater than 10 out of 21 or an Overall Anxiety Severity and Impairment Scale (OASIS)28 score greater than 7 out of 20. Selected psychotropic agents (e.g., serotonergic/noradrenergic) were allowed so long as they were stably medicated (no changes within four weeks). We report data that was collected as part of a larger fMRI study that included an interoceptive perturbation task (isoproterenol infusion) performed after collection of the resting data presented here29. Since the larger study focuses on psychiatric disorders that predominantly occur in females (e.g., GAD, anorexia nervosa), the sample base for this investigation was also female-only. Further details on the aforementioned study can be found on the ClinicalTrials.gov registration (NCT02615119). All participants were administered the Patient Health Questionnaire depression module (PHQ-9)30, the GAD-7 questionnaire31, the OASIS28, the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)32, and the Anxiety Sensitivity Index (ASI)33. HCs were required to be without any history of psychiatric illness per the MINI interview. The HC group was individually matched to the GAD group so that they would not differ significantly on body mass index (BMI) and age due to the known influence of the former on head motion34 and the latter on FC35. Further details on inclusion criteria and selection of participants (including CONSORT diagram) are listed in the Supplement.
The study was approved by the Western institutional review board and was conducted at the Laureate Institute for Brain Research. All participants provided written informed consent and received financial compensation for their study involvement.
Image acquisition
Magnetic resonance images were obtained using two identical full-body 3.0 Tesla MR750 MRI scanners (GE Healthcare, Milwaukee, WI), equipped with an 8-channel head array coil (GE Healthcare, Milwaukee, WI). First, a T1-weighted image was acquired as an anatomical reference, followed by an 8-min resting-state scan using a single-shot gradient-recalled echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence (see Supplementary Methods for details). Prior to the resting-state scan, participants were instructed to remain as still as possible, to keep their eyes open and fixated on a cross presented at the center of the screen, and to “clear your mind and do not think about anything in particular”.
Data processing
Preprocessing
Preprocessing of fMRI data was conducted using AFNI 20.0.19 (RRID:SCR_005927)23 and Freesurfer 6.0.0 (RRID:SCR_001847)36. T1-weighted images were skull stripped and nonlinearly warped to Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) 152 atlas space. Physiological noise effects (i.e., due to cardiac pulsatility and respiration) were regressed out using the RETROICOR method37 implemented in AFNI. Volumes censored due to too much motion or being signal outliers were interpolated using the previous and subsequent volume. Participants displaying excessive motion or signal outliers during their resting scan (i.e., > 30% volumes being censored because of motion or signal outliers) were excluded. See Supplement for further data preprocessing details.
Region of interest definition and data extraction
Based on a careful review of the fMRI literature on GAD we assessed FC between a total of nine regions of interest (ROIs). We then formulated a total of 11 a-priori hypotheses about FC between the nine pre-defined ROIs for examination (Table 1). To extract the data for each ROI, a mask was created by collapsing over the relevant labels of the Brainnetome atlas38, which provides a probabilistic cytoarchitectonic parcellation of the human brain. The average timeseries for each ROI was then extracted for each participant.
Statistical analyses
Using the timeseries of the nine ROIs we constructed a 9 × 9 correlation matrix for each participant. The relationship between ROIs was assessed using Pearson’s correlation. The resulting sampling distribution of Pearson’s r was normalized using the Fisher r-to-z transformation and the obtained z-scores were used in all further analyses.
Bayesian modeling
A Bayesian multilevel model (BML)24 was applied to the data using the MBA program in AFNI, estimating the posterior probability of the effect being greater than 0 (P+). The BML was also used to explore all other possible region pairs that we did not hypothesize a-priori to be aberrant in GAD, and therefore left out of the FC analysis. The BML overcomes limitations of NHST in this context by (a) incorporating the interrelationships between region pairs into one model through partial pooling, (b) addressing the issue of multiplicity in the conventional NHST framework, (c) providing direct evidence for or against the effect of a region pair instead of assuming the null-hypothesis46, and (d) estimating the contribution of each individual brain region in the network relative to all other regions as a measurement of “relative importance”. Additionally, the BML inherently supports full result reporting and treats statistical evidence as a continuum instead of arbitrarily dichotomizing results as “significant” or “not significant”. While acknowledging that measures of uncertainty (e.g., confidence intervals) and effect sizes can also be derived in the NHST framework, results obtained with frequentist inference are often primarily evaluated by their p-value47. For a formal explanation of how the BML is used to estimate a posterior probability distribution please refer to Chen et al.24.
Mass univariate analysis
Welch’s independent samples t-tests were used to test the null hypothesis that there was no difference in FC-scores between the two groups. To prevent the inflation of Type I errors (i.e., the problem of multiplicity) the results were Bonferroni corrected. The conservative Bonferroni correction was chosen over other frequent multiplicity correction methods in the field (i.e., family-wise error rate or false discovery rate) to improve the validity of the results and to facilitate a meaningful comparison with the BML approach. To decrease the likelihood of committing Type II errors, only the region pairs hypothesized to be aberrant in GAD (Table 1) were tested and consequently the Bonferroni corrected α-value for each hypothesis was adjusted based on how often the data of a ROI was used in multiple comparisons (for further details on multiplicity correction see Supplement). Finally, exploratory relationships between FC and symptom scores were examined using Pearson’s correlation, Bonferroni corrected for multiple comparisons, and corresponding Bayes Factors (BF) were calculated using the “BayesFactor” package in R.
Ethical standards
The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.
Results
Sample characteristics
Demographic and clinical information of the female study sample are summarized in Table 2. The participants did not differ significantly on age, BMI, or average head motion (see Supplement). As expected, the GAD group exhibited higher psychopathology scores on the PHQ-9, OASIS, GAD-7, STAI, and ASI questionnaires (Table 2).
Resting-state fMRI analysis
Bayesian multilevel modeling
The results obtained from administering the BML24 to our data identified several region pairs with strong evidence for the group difference being greater (or less) than 0, most notably the vmPFC-PMI region pair (P+ = 0.98). The BML indicated strong evidence for a group difference for some of the region pairs that were also tested in the NHST model (i.e., dlPFC-amygdala, P+ = 0.96; vmPFC-AI, P+ = 0.95; PMI-amygdala, P+ = 0.95).
Interestingly, the BML analysis identified other region pairs to have reduced FC that were not hypothesized a-priori and were therefore not examined in the NHST analysis. These included the dlPFC-PMI, the vmPFC-dACC, the dmPFC-PMI, the dlPFC-dACC, the TP-PMI, and the vmPFC-dmPFC region pairs, all of which indicated high probabilities for a group difference. The complete results of the BML for all region pairs are illustrated in Fig. 1.
The finding of the vmPFC-PMI region pair showing strong evidence for a group difference in the BML was reinforced by individual region effect estimates for the group comparisons: Both the vmPFC (P+ = 0.972) and the PMI (P+ = 0.972) showed the highest posterior probabilities for observing a region effect in the HC minus GAD contrast of all areas included in the BML. The complete list of region effects and their respective probabilities are shown in Fig. 2.
Mass-univariate analysis
Results from the conventional mass-univariate analysis revealed that participants in the GAD group had significantly lower FC compared to HCs between the vmPFC and PMI, the vmPFC and AI, the amygdala and PMI, and the amygdala and dlPFC region pairs. However, after Bonferroni-correction of all hypotheses tested, only the vmPFC and PMI result remained significant (Fig. 3). This finding is in line with the top result from the BML, that convergently identified the vmPFC-PMI region pair to have the highest probability for a group difference. Detailed results for all 11 hypotheses tested can be found in Table 3.
Exploring the relationship between vmPFC-PMI connectivity and symptom scores
To investigate whether the reduced FC between the vmPFC and PMI, observed in both frequentist and Bayesian analyses, was related to greater psychopathology, we calculated Pearson’s r between vmPFC-PMI z-scores and clinical scores assessed by the following validated questionnaires: The GAD-7, the PHQ-9, the ASI, the STAI, and the OASIS. The correlation between vmPFC-PMI z-score and the ASI Total score was statistically significant only in the GAD group and before multiplicity correction but the Bayes Factor indicates that the presence of correlation between the vmPFC-PMI z-score and the ASI Total score was 3 times more likely than the absence of correlation (r = − 0.42, 95% CI [− 0.69, − 0.05], p = 0.029, padj = 0.175, BF = 3.03, Fig. S2). All other correlations with clinical scores were statistically non-significant even before multiplicity correction (Table 4) and their Bayes Factors indicated the absence of correlation.
Discussion
In this preregistered study, we examined resting state FC in females with GAD relative to matched HCs to test a set of a-priori hypotheses using dual statistical frameworks: Bayesian multilevel modeling and NHST. Converging results from both analyses confirmed diminished FC between the PMI and the vmPFC in the GAD group compared to HCs. FC between these regions was associated with one clinically relevant trait measure, anxiety sensitivity, in the GAD group.
Examining the data using Bayesian multilevel modeling overcomes common problems with NHST47 (see Supplementary Discussion) while providing evidence for abnormal FC among region pairs that were not hypothesized a-priori and were therefore not tested in the NHST analysis. This included evidence of decreased FC among several regions including the dlPFC-PMI, the vmPFC-dACC, the dmPFC-PMI, the dlPFC-dACC, the TP-PMI, and the vmPFC-dmPFC. Thus, the application of both statistical frameworks and approaching neural connectivity data in GAD from two different statistical viewpoints yielded confirmatory results (most notably vmPFC-PMI) and provided indications of other relationships worth examining further.
Both the insula and the vmPFC are brain regions relevant to cognitive and emotional processing and have consequently been examined in previous investigations of the pathophysiology of anxiety disorders. Numerous studies have implicated the vmPFC in decision making48, generation and regulation of emotion49, and fear conditioning50. In GAD, the vmPFC has been previously associated with greater fear generalization39, which fits the clinical picture of excessive worry in individuals with the disorder1. Moreover, abnormal vmPFC functioning has most often been implicated in anxiety disorders in regards to its proposed role of inhibiting amygdala output51 (but see a different perspective by Myers-Schulz and Koenigs52). This seems reasonable considering the widely accepted view of the amygdala as a central hub for fear processing53. However, several lines of evidence show the need to distinguish between exteroceptive fear processing, which is most prominently mediated through the amygdala, and interoceptive fear processing, which is most prominently mediated through the insular cortex. For example, studies of individuals with bilateral amygdala lesions have shown a remarkable absence of anxiety or panic in response to exteroceptive fear stimuli (e.g., visual stimuli like snakes, spiders, or film clips)54 but experienced fear and panic evoked by interoceptive stimuli (e.g., carbon dioxide inhalation causing dyspnea or β-adrenergic agonist infusion causing palpitations)55.
Interoception is a construct tightly linked to the insular cortex (among other regions including the medial prefrontal cortex and amygdala)56 and encompasses the processing of internal body signals by the nervous system57. Models of interoceptive processing have suggested a posterior-to-anterior integration of interoceptive signaling within the human insula58,59 that is supported by its pattern of cytoarchitectonic organization with an agranular rostral and dysgranular/granular mid and posterior divisions across humans and primates60,61. Studies examining the functional organization of the insula implicate the AI in task maintenance62, attention control63, emotion64, and predictive processing59, which is in line with increased insula activity during emotional processing tasks in anxiety-prone individuals65. Recent results from our group collected in the same sample show blunted vmPFC activity during an interoceptive perturbation task (pharmacologic infusions of a fast-acting peripheral adrenaline analog resulting in cardiorespiratory modulation29), a method that has been reliably shown to activate the insula66.
The results from this current study suggest the implication of the vmPFC and insula as networked brain regions in the pathophysiology of GAD. More precisely, reduced vmPFC-PMI FC could support the idea that individuals with GAD may have difficulty exercising top-down regulation of emotion due to aberrant processing of bottom-up signals flowing through an interoceptive hub: the insula. This hypothesis is backed by our observations of vmPFC and PMI differences between HC and GAD, which were confirmed by both statistical approaches. While reduced vmPFCI-PMI FC at rest could partly be explained by increased sensitivity of the insula to interoceptive events in the GAD group, it seems plausible that impaired prefrontal regulation of negatively valenced interoceptive states plays a stronger role in this connection based on the observation of vmPFC hypoactivation during the aforementioned cardiorespiratory perturbation task in the same sample29. We also found that FC between the vmPFC and PMI was negatively associated with anxiety sensitivity, which is broadly defined as the fear of experiencing anxiety-related sensations especially those arising from within the body (e.g., heart palpitations or dyspnea)67. In a clinical context, this could mean that the smaller the correlated activity between the vmPFC and the insula at rest, the more likely patients are to experience internal body states as anxiety provoking. However, this interpretation is preliminary and other clinical scores were not correlated with vmPFC-PMI FC, suggesting that this relation might be specific to the anxiety sensitivity construct. Also, this result was statistically significant only before Bonferroni correction and while the Bayes factor indicated that a relationship between vmPFC-insula FC and anxiety sensitivity is likely, our dual statistical approach did not converge on this result. In conclusion, this finding provides some initial evidence of functional association between abnormal neural activity in the vmPFC and PMI and a transdiagnostic trait underlying the initiation and maintenance of pathological anxiety68.
Other results from the frequentist analysis indicated abnormal FC of the amygdala in GAD. Though contrary to our hypothesis, we observed decreased rather than increased FC between the amygdala and the PMI. The direction of this finding also contrasts with previous reports of an amygdala-insula resting state network in both anxious adults69 and adolescents15, but on the other hand aligns with other previous findings of reduced amygdala-insula FC13. Additionally, FC between the amygdala and the dlPFC was decreased, not increased, in our GAD sample. This finding was against our hypothesis that was based on previous literature13. Decreased FC between the amygdala and the dlPFC, which is a central node in the CEN, could be argued to reflect a dysfunctional management of attention (a key function of the CEN9) towards threat-related stimuli, which is a key clinical feature of GAD70. However, the overly general view of the amygdala as the central hub of fear processing is challenged by the absence of amygdala involvement in human fear extinction in a recent meta-analysis71, and heterogenous amygdala findings across reviews of neuroimaging literature in GAD5,72. While the results from our cross-sectional study might hint at the possibility that the role of the amygdala might not be as pivotal to the maintenance of GAD as expected, both amygdala-related findings (i.e., reduced FC for the PMI-amygdala and the dlPFC-amygdala in the GAD group) did not withstand correction for multiplicity and would therefore not be considered statistically significant using the NHST model framework. On the other hand, evaluation of the results from the BML indicated high probabilities for a group difference regarding those region pairs, raising the question whether overly rigorous multiplicity correction might have induced a type II error in our NHST-analysis of those brain regions. Viewing the data from a different, i.e., Bayesian, perspective thus strengthened the validity of our reduced amygdala FC findings, permitting us to discuss these results and consider their potential implications for GAD. Further insight into FC of the amygdala (and more generally, all of the selected ROIs) could be gained by employing seed-based whole-brain voxel wise FC analysis, a common approach to identify the networked connectivity of brain regions73. However, large datasets are required with this method to have sufficient statistical power and consequently, efforts have been made by the ENIGMA consortium to provide an analysis pipeline for employing seed-based FC analysis on pooled datasets from multicenter studies74 that can provide such large sample sizes.
Bayesian multilevel modeling further allowed us to investigate relationships in GAD that were not hypothesized a-priori with minimal risk of information loss. Our analysis identified high probabilities for decreased FC of the PMI with the dlPFC, the dmPFC, and the TP. Decreased functional coupling of the PMI and the dlPFC could be interpreted to reflect abnormal signaling of internal body systems to a key region for executive functions like working memory75 and attention76: aspects of cognition known to be impaired in anxiety77,78. The reduced PMI FC between both the dmPFC (a brain area known to be hyperactivated in GAD during emotional processing17 and at rest79), and the TP (an area implicated in social and emotional processing80), align well with a proposed model of the insula as an “integral hub” for detecting salient events, and for switching attention to these stimuli in preparation for regulatory (i.e., visceromotor) processing81. These additional findings suggest that the insula shows decreased functional coupling at rest with brain areas that have previously been found to show aberrant activity and/or connectivity in anxious individuals and whose functions are relevant to the clinical characteristics of GAD. However, this interpretation remains preliminary and requires causal examination in further experiments.
The Bayesian multilevel analysis also revealed diminished FC of the vmPFC-dmPFC region pair in GAD, two key components of the DMN7. This finding is consistent with previous reports of DMN alterations in GAD12, albeit decreased FC between the vmPFC and dmPFC has not been reported previously. These regions of the DMN are hypothesized to promote functions like processing of emotion and self-referential cognition7, which are impaired in GAD82. Lastly, the Bayesian analysis revealed reduced FC with the vmPFC and the dlPFC, which are key components, respectively, of the DMN and CEN networks8. Additionally, the GAD group exhibited decreased FC of both these regions with the dACC, a key node in the salience network and hypothesized to facilitate “switching” between the spontaneous cognition of the DMN83 and executive functioning of the CEN8. These results hint at the possibility that decreased FC between the vmPFC and dlPFC could be mediated by reduced functional coupling of these regions to the dACC.
Limitations
Limitations of this study include a female-only sample with modest size (that is still above average compared to fMRI studies in recent years84), selected psychotropic medication allowance, and the methodological limitation that correlational analysis cannot determine the causality or directionality (i.e., responsible region) for impaired FC observed within region pairs (see Supplement for further discussion). The choice of brain regions we investigated was based on previous literature, but is not exhaustive. Other brain areas relevant to pathological mechanisms in GAD (e.g., thalamus85 or striatum44,86) should be investigated in future studies. As mentioned in the “Discussion”, testing FC differences between region pairs does not allow for network analysis as commonly employed in seed-based FC analysis across the whole brain. Our focus on females with GAD was based on the fact that females outnumber males with the disorder by a factor of two to one87, and that our sample was drawn from a larger study examining psychiatric disorders predominantly affecting females (e.g., anorexia nervosa and GAD). Future research is needed to establish whether our findings extend to males, i.e., whether sex differences in FC play a role in GAD. A recent mega-analysis found structural brain differences only in males with GAD but no general effect of GAD on brain structure88, indicating that a dynamic approach using functional MRI could provide better insight into the neurobiology of GAD.
Conclusion
We leveraged the strengths of the Bayesian inference framework to convergently identify reduced FC between the vmPFC and the PMI in GAD and identified an association of this relationship with the anxiety sensitivity trait. Bayesian multilevel modeling allowed us to identify decreased FC between region pairs excluded by the frequentist analysis and other previously undescribed regions, emphasizing the utility of this method for probing the pathophysiological basis of psychiatric disorders. Future fMRI studies of resting state FC may benefit from a similar approach.
Data availability
All study data and scripts necessary to replicate the results of this study are available online on the Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/vf7s4/.
References
Hoge, E. A., Ivkovic, A. & Fricchione, G. L. Generalized anxiety disorder: Diagnosis and treatment. BMJ 345, e7500 (2012).
Stein, M. B. Neurobiology of generalized anxiety disorder. J. Clin. Psychiatry 70, 15–19 (2009).
Fonzo, G. A. & Etkin, A. Affective neuroimaging in generalized anxiety disorder: An integrated review. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 19, 169–179 (2017).
van den Heuvel, M. P. & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. Exploring the brain network: A review on resting-state fMRI functional connectivity. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 20, 519–534 (2010).
Hilbert, K., Lueken, U. & Beesdo-Baum, K. Neural structures, functioning and connectivity in Generalized Anxiety Disorder and interaction with neuroendocrine systems: A systematic review. J. Affect. Disord. 158, 114–126 (2014).
Kolesar, T. A., Bilevicius, E., Wilson, A. D. & Kornelsen, J. Systematic review and meta-analyses of neural structural and functional differences in generalized anxiety disorder and healthy controls using magnetic resonance imaging. NeuroImage Clin. 24, 102016 (2019).
Raichle, M. E. et al. A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 98, 676–682 (2001).
Seeley, W. W. et al. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27, 2349–2356 (2007).
Bressler, S. L. & Menon, V. Large-scale brain networks in cognition: Emerging methods and principles. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 277–290 (2010).
Rabany, L. et al. Resting-state functional connectivity in generalized anxiety disorder and social anxiety disorder: Evidence for a dimensional approach. Brain Connect. 7, 289–298 (2017).
Xiong, H., Guo, R.-J. & Shi, H.-W. Altered default mode network and salience network functional connectivity in patients with generalized anxiety disorders: An ICA-based resting-state fMRI study. Evid. Based Complement. Alternat. Med. 2020, e4048916 (2020).
Andreescu, C., Sheu, L. K., Tudorascu, D., Walker, S. & Aizenstein, H. The ages of anxiety-differences across the lifespan in the default mode network functional connectivity in generalized anxiety disorder: The ages of anxiety. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 29, 704–712 (2014).
Etkin, A., Prater, K. E., Schatzberg, A. F., Menon, V. & Greicius, M. D. Disrupted Amygdalar subregion functional connectivity and evidence of a compensatory network in generalized anxiety disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 66, 1361–1372 (2009).
Cui, H. et al. Insula shows abnormal task-evoked and resting-state activity in first-episode drug-naïve generalized anxiety disorder. Depress. Anxiety 37, 632–644 (2020).
Roy, A. K. et al. Intrinsic functional connectivity of amygdala-based networks in adolescent generalized anxiety disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 52, 290-299.e2 (2013).
Li, W. et al. Aberrant functional connectivity between the amygdala and the temporal pole in drug-free generalized anxiety disorder. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10, 549 (2016).
Paulesu, E. et al. Neural correlates of worry in generalized anxiety disorder and in normal controls: A functional MRI study. Psychol. Med. 40, 117–124 (2010).
Ball, T. M., Ramsawh, H. J., Campbell-Sills, L., Paulus, M. P. & Stein, M. B. Prefrontal dysfunction during emotion regulation in generalized anxiety and panic disorders. Psychol. Med. 43, 1475–1486 (2013).
Chen, G. Sources of information waste in neuroimaging: mishandling structures, thinking dichotomously, and over-reducing data. Aperture Neuro 2, 1–22 (2022).
Maxwell, S. E., Kelley, K. & Rausch, J. R. Sample size planning for statistical power and accuracy in parameter estimation. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59, 537–563 (2008).
Smith, S. & Nichols, T. Threshold-free cluster enhancement: Addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. Neuroimage 44, 83–98 (2009).
Wagenmakers, E.-J. et al. Bayesian inference for psychology. Part I: Theoretical advantages and practical ramifications. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 25, 35–57 (2018).
Cox, R. W. AFNI: Software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 29, 162–173 (1996).
Chen, G. et al. An integrative Bayesian approach to matrix-based analysis in neuroimaging. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40, 4072–4090 (2019).
Steinhäuser, J., Teed, A. & Khalsa, S. Correlated activity in generalized anxiety disorder—a resting-state fMRI approach. (2020) https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/J29QV.
Sheehan, D. V. et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry 59(Suppl 20), 22–33 (1998).
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. (2013).
Campbell-Sills, L. et al. Validation of a brief measure of anxiety-related severity and impairment: The Overall Anxiety Severity and Impairment Scale (OASIS). J. Affect. Disord. 112, 92–101 (2009).
Teed, A. R. et al. Association of generalized anxiety disorder with autonomic hypersensitivity and blunted ventromedial prefrontal cortex activity during peripheral adrenergic stimulation: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiat. 79, 323 (2022).
Williams, N. PHQ-9. Occup. Med. 64, 139–140 (2014).
Spitzer, R. L., Kroenke, K., Williams, J. B. W. & Löwe, B. A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: The GAD-7. Arch. Intern. Med. 166, 1092 (2006).
Spielberger, C., Gorsuch, R., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. & Jacobs, G. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory Vol. IV (Consulting Psychologists Press, 1983).
Reiss, S., Peterson, R. A., Gursky, D. M. & McNally, R. J. Anxiety sensitivity, anxiety frequency and the prediction of fearfulness. Behav. Res. Ther. 24, 1–8 (1986).
Ekhtiari, H., Kuplicki, R., Yeh, H. & Paulus, M. P. Physical characteristics not psychological state or trait characteristics predict motion during resting state fMRI. Sci. Rep. 9, 419 (2019).
Betzel, R. F. et al. Changes in structural and functional connectivity among resting-state networks across the human lifespan. Neuroimage 102, 345–357 (2014).
Fischl, B. FreeSurfer. Neuroimage 62, 774–781 (2012).
Glover, G. H., Li, T. Q. & Ress, D. Image-based method for retrospective correction of physiological motion effects in fMRI: RETROICOR. Magn. Reson. Med. 44, 162–167 (2000).
Fan, L. et al. The Human Brainnetome Atlas: A new brain atlas based on connectional architecture. Cereb. Cortex 26, 3508–3526 (2016).
Greenberg, T., Carlson, J. M., Cha, J., Hajcak, G. & Mujica-Parodi, L. R. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex reactivity is altered in generalized anxiety disorder during fear generalization. Depress. Anxiety 30, 242–250 (2013).
Toazza, R. et al. Amygdala-based intrinsic functional connectivity and anxiety disorders in adolescents and young adults. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 257, 11–16 (2016).
Makovac, E. et al. Alterations in amygdala-prefrontal functional connectivity account for excessive worry and autonomic dysregulation in generalized anxiety disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 80, 786–795 (2016).
Mohlman, J., Eldreth, D. A., Price, R. B., Staples, A. M. & Hanson, C. Prefrontal-limbic connectivity during worry in older adults with generalized anxiety disorder. Aging Ment. Health 21, 426–438 (2017).
Porta-Casteràs, D. et al. Prefrontal-amygdala connectivity in trait anxiety and generalized anxiety disorder: Testing the boundaries between healthy and pathological worries. J. Affect. Disord. 267, 211–219 (2020).
Qiao, J. et al. Aberrant functional network connectivity as a biomarker of generalized anxiety disorder. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11, 626 (2017).
Liu, W. et al. Abnormal functional connectivity of the amygdala-based network in resting-state fMRI in adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder. Med. Sci. Monit. 21, 459–467 (2015).
Chen, G. et al. Handling multiplicity in neuroimaging through Bayesian lenses with multilevel modeling. Neuroinformatics 17, 515–545 (2019).
Wasserstein, R. L. & Lazar, N. A. The ASA statement on p-values: Context, process, and purpose. Am. Stat. 70, 129–133 (2016).
Bechara, A., Tranel, D. & Damasio, H. Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain 123, 2189–2202 (2000).
Hiser, J. & Koenigs, M. The multifaceted role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in emotion, decision making, social cognition, and psychopathology. Biol. Psychiatry 83, 638–647 (2018).
Sotres-Bayon, F., Cain, C. K. & LeDoux, J. E. Brain mechanisms of fear extinction: Historical perspectives on the contribution of prefrontal cortex. Biol. Psychiatry 60, 329–336 (2006).
Phelps, E. A., Delgado, M. R., Nearing, K. I. & LeDoux, J. E. Extinction learning in humans. Neuron 43, 897–905 (2004).
Myers-Schulz, B. & Koenigs, M. Functional anatomy of ventromedial prefrontal cortex: Implications for mood and anxiety disorders. Mol. Psychiatry 17, 132–141 (2012).
LeDoux, J. The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 23, 727–738 (2003).
Feinstein, J. S., Adolphs, R., Damasio, A. & Tranel, D. The human amygdala and the induction and experience of fear. Curr. Biol. 21, 34–38 (2011).
Khalsa, S. S. et al. Panic anxiety in humans with bilateral amygdala lesions: Pharmacological induction via cardiorespiratory interoceptive pathways. J. Neurosci. 36, 3559–3566 (2016).
Berntson, G. G. & Khalsa, S. S. Neural circuits of interoception. Trends Neurosci. 44, 17–28 (2021).
Khalsa, S. S. et al. Interoception and mental health: A roadmap. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 3, 501–513 (2018).
Craig, A. D. How do you feel–now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 59–70 (2009).
Barrett, L. F. & Simmons, W. K. Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 419–429 (2015).
Morel, A., Gallay, M. N., Baechler, A., Wyss, M. & Gallay, D. S. The human insula: Architectonic organization and postmortem MRI registration. Neuroscience 236, 117–135 (2013).
Evrard, H. C., Logothetis, N. K. & Craig, A. D. B. Modular architectonic organization of the insula in the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 522, 64–97 (2014).
Dosenbach, N. U. F. et al. A core system for the implementation of task sets. Neuron 50, 799–812 (2006).
Nelson, S. M. et al. Role of the anterior insula in task-level control and focal attention. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 669–680 (2010).
Zaki, J., Davis, J. I. & Ochsner, K. N. Overlapping activity in anterior insula during interoception and emotional experience. Neuroimage 62, 493–499 (2012).
Stein, M. B., Simmons, A. N., Feinstein, J. S. & Paulus, M. P. Increased amygdala and insula activation during emotion processing in anxiety-prone subjects. Am. J. Psychiatry 164, 318–327 (2007).
Hassanpour, M. S. et al. The insular cortex dynamically maps changes in cardiorespiratory interoception. Neuropsychopharmacology 43, 426–434 (2018).
Anxiety Sensitivity: Theory, Research, and Treatment of the Fear of Anxiety. (Routledge, 2014). https://doi.org/10.4324/9781410603326.
Naragon-Gainey, K. Meta-analysis of the relations of anxiety sensitivity to the depressive and anxiety disorders. Psychol. Bull. 136, 128–150 (2010).
Baur, V., Hänggi, J., Langer, N. & Jäncke, L. Resting-state functional and structural connectivity within an insula-amygdala route specifically index state and trait anxiety. Biol. Psychiatry 73, 85–92 (2013).
Mogg, K. & Bradley, B. P. Attentional bias in generalized anxiety disorder versus depressive disorder. Cogn. Ther. Res. 29, 29–45 (2005).
Fullana, M. A. et al. Fear extinction in the human brain: A meta-analysis of fMRI studies in healthy participants. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 88, 16–25 (2018).
Mochcovitch, M. D., da Rocha Freire, R. C., Garcia, R. F. & Nardi, A. E. A systematic review of fMRI studies in generalized anxiety disorder: Evaluating its neural and cognitive basis. J. Affect. Disord. 167, 336–342 (2014).
Cole, D., Smith, S. & Beckmann, C. Advances and pitfalls in the analysis and interpretation of resting-state FMRI data. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4 (2010).
Adhikari, B. M. et al. A resting state fMRI analysis pipeline for pooling inference across diverse cohorts: An ENIGMA rs-fMRI protocol. Brain Imaging Behav. 13, 1453–1467. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2010.00008 (2019).
Barbey, A. K., Koenigs, M. & Grafman, J. Dorsolateral prefrontal contributions to human working memory. Cortex 49, 1195–1205 (2013).
Kane, M. J. & Engle, R. W. The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspective. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 9, 637–671 (2002).
Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. & van IJzendoorn, M. H. Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychol. Bull. 133, 1–24 (2007).
Vytal, K. E., Cornwell, B. R., Letkiewicz, A. M., Arkin, N. E. & Grillon, C. The complex interaction between anxiety and cognition: Insight from spatial and verbal working memory. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 93 (2013).
Wang, W. et al. Aberrant regional neural fluctuations and functional connectivity in generalized anxiety disorder revealed by resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neurosci. Lett. 624, 78–84 (2016).
Olson, I. R., Plotzker, A. & Ezzyat, Y. The enigmatic temporal pole: A review of findings on social and emotional processing. Brain 130, 1718–1731 (2007).
Menon, V. & Uddin, L. Q. Saliency, switching, attention and control: A network model of insula function. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 655–667 (2010).
Turk, C. L., Heimberg, R. G., Luterek, J. A., Mennin, D. S. & Fresco, D. M. Emotion dysregulation in generalized anxiety disorder: A comparison with social anxiety disorder. Cogn. Ther. Res. 29, 89–106 (2005).
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Huang, C. & Buckner, R. L. Evidence for the default network’s role in spontaneous cognition. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 322–335 (2010).
Szucs, D. & Ioannidis, J. P. A. Sample size evolution in neuroimaging research: An evaluation of highly-cited studies (1990–2012) and of latest practices (2017–2018) in high-impact journals. Neuroimage 221, 117164 (2020).
Buff, C. et al. Directed threat imagery in generalized anxiety disorder. Psychol. Med. 48, 617–628 (2018).
White, S. F. et al. Prediction error representation in individuals with generalized anxiety disorder during passive avoidance. Am. J. Psychiatry 174, 110–117 (2017).
Weisberg, R. B. Overview of generalized anxiety disorder: Epidemiology, presentation, and course. J. Clin. Psychiatry 70(Suppl 2), 4–9 (2009).
Harrewijn, A. et al. Cortical and subcortical brain structure in generalized anxiety disorder: Findings from 28 research sites in the ENIGMA-Anxiety Working Group. Transl. Psychiatry 11, 502 (2021).
Acknowledgements
The authors wish to acknowledge the support of Valerie Upshaw MSN, APRN-CNP in gathering participants information and Rayus Kuplicki PhD for help with MRI data management.
Funding
This work was supported by National Institute of General Medical Sciences (NIGMS) Center Grant P20GM121312 (S.S.K.), National Institute of Mental Health Grants K23MH112949 and R01MH127225 (S.S.K.), The William K. Warren Foundation (S.S.K.) and the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research by providing J.S. with a scholarship for his collaboration with the Laureate Institute for Brain Research. The views expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the position or policy of the National Institutes of Health.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
J.L.S.: conceptualization, methodology, software, formal analysis, investigation, data curation, writing—original draft, visualization. A.R.T.: conceptualization, data curation, writing—review and editing. O.A.Z: software, data curation, writing—review and editing. R.H.: investigation, writing—review and editing. G.C.: methodology, software, writing—review and editing, visualization. S.S.K.: conceptualization, investigation, data curation, resources, writing—original draft, review, and editing, visualization, supervision, project administration, funding acquisition.
Corresponding authors
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Steinhäuser, J.L., Teed, A.R., Al-Zoubi, O. et al. Reduced vmPFC-insula functional connectivity in generalized anxiety disorder: a Bayesian confirmation study. Sci Rep 13, 9626 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-35939-2
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-35939-2