Abstract
There is a shortfall of girls and women pursuing STEM disciplines, a deficit that may be partially attributed to subtle forms of bias that are tied to traditional gender role stereotypes. The current study examined these subtle biases in high school teachers and students in two ways: by asking teachers and students to attribute masculine and feminine traits to the typical scientist or humanities professional, and by inquiring about the academic performance assumed of boys and girls. In addition, students were surveyed about their own self-efficacy in math and science courses and gender-balanced teaching initiatives present in the classroom. Results showed that teachers and students exhibited subtle bias by attributing more masculine characteristics to a scientist and feminine characteristics to the humanities. Teachers and students also reported their belief that boys tend to perform better than girls in STEM disciplines. Finally, our results echoed previous literature examining self-efficacy discrepancies for girls in math and science classes, and indicated a lack of gender-balanced teaching initiatives in math and science classes. These results have broad implications for the lack of women in STEM disciplines, and they also reveal a potential means of classroom intervention to help counteract these subtle forms of bias.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Beede, D., Julian, T., Langdon, D., McKitrick, G., Khan, B., & Doms, M. (2011). Women in STEM: A gender gap to innovation. Economics and Statistics Administration Issue Brief, 4(11), 1–11.
Bettinger, E. P., & Long, B. T. (2005). Do faculty serve as role models? The impact of instructor gender on female students. Understanding Teacher Quality, 95(2), 152–157.
Carli, L. L., Alawa, L., Lee, Y., Zhao, B., & Kim, E. (2016). Stereotypes about gender and science women ≠ scientists. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 40(2), 244–260. doi:10.1177/0361684315622645
Charles, M., & Bradley, K. (2002). Equal but separate? A cross-national study of sex segregation in higher education. American Sociological Review, 67, 573–599.
Charles, M., & Bradley, K. (2009). Indulging our gendered selves? Sex segregation by field of study in 44 countries. American Journal of Sociology, 114, 924–976.
Cortina, J. M. (1993). What is coefficient alpha? An examination of theory and applications. Psychometrika, 16, 297–334.
Diekman, A. B., & Eagly, A. H. (2000). Stereotypes as dynamic constructs: Women and men of the past, present, and future. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(10), 1171–1188.
Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior: A social-role interpretation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychological Review, 109, 573–598.
Eagly, A. H., & Sczesny, S. (2009). Stereotypes about women, men, and leaders: Have times changed? In M. Barreto, M.K. Ryan, & M.T. Schmitt (Eds.), The glass ceiling in the 21st century: understanding barriers to gender equality (pp. 21–47). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Erikson, E. H. (1994). Identity: Youth and crisis (No. 7). New York City, NY: WW Norton & Company.
Fennema, E., Peterson, P. L., Carpenter, T. P., & Lubinski, C. A. (1990). Teachers’ attributions and beliefs about girls, boys, and mathematics. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 21, 460–507.
Halpern, D. F. (2013). Sex differences in cognitive abilities. Hove: Psychology Press.
Hazari, Z., Sadler, P. M., & Tai, R. H. (2008). Gender differences in the high school and affective experiences of introductory college physics students. The Physics Teacher, 46(7), 423–427.
Maxwell, S. E., & Delaney, H. D. (2004). Designing experiments and analyzing data: A model comparison perspective (Vol. 1). Hove: Psychology Press.
Michigan Study of Adolescent and Adult Life Transitions. (2006). University of Michigan Gender and Achievement Research Program. Retrieved from http://www.rcgd.isr.umich.edu/msalt.
Moss-Racusin, C. A., Dovido, J. F., Brescoll, V. L., Graham, M. J., & Handlesman, J. (2012). Science faculty’s subtle gender biases favor male students. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(41), 16474–16479.
Moss-Racusin, C. A., Molenda, A. K., & Cramer, C. R. (2015). Can evidence impact attitudes? Public reactions to evidence of gender bias in STEM fields. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 1(16), 1–16.
National Science Foundation. (2014). Science and engineering indicators. Retrieved from http://www.nsf.gov/statistics/seind14/.
Penner, A., & CadwalladerOlsker, T. (2012). Gender differences in mathematics and science achievement across the distribution: What international variation can tell us about the role of biology and society. In F. D. Rivera & H. Forgasz (Eds.), Toward equity in mathematics education: Gender, culture, and diversity (pp. 441–468). New York: Springer.
Phelan, J. E., Moss-Racusin, C. A., & Rudman, L. A. (2008). Competent yet out in the cold: Shifting criteria for hiring reflect backlash toward agentic women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32(4), 406–413.
Potvin, G., Hazari, Z., Tai, R. H., & Sadler, P. M. (2009). Unraveling bias from student evaluations of their high school science teachers. Science Education, 93(5), 827–845.
Reigle-Crumb, C., & Humpries, M. (2012). Exploring bias in math teachers’ perceptions of students’ ability by gender and race/ethnicity. National Institute of Health and Public Access, 26(2), 1–17.
Rice, L., & Barth, J. M. (2016). Hiring decisions: The effect of evaluator gender and gender stereotype characteristics on the evaluation of job applicants. Gender Issues, 33(1), 1–21.
Rice, L., Barth, J. M., Guadagno, R. E., Smith, G. P. A., & McCallum, D. M. (2013). The role of social support in student’s perceived abilities and attitudes toward math and science. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42(7), 1028–1040.
Riegle-Crumb, C. (2012). The cross-national context of the gender gap in math and science. In L. Hedges & B. Schneider (Eds.), The social organization of schools (pp. 227–243). New York: Russell Sage Press.
Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Barquissau, M. (2004). The costs of accepting gender differences: The role of stereotype endorsement in women’s experience in the math domain. Sex Roles, 50(11–12), 835–850.
Shettle, C., Roey, S., Mordica, J., Perkins, R., Nord, C., Tedorovic, J., et al. (2007). The nation’s report card: America’s high school graduates. Results from the 2005 NAEP High School Transcript Study. Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics. Retrieved from http://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/pdf/studies/2007467.pdf.
Stout, J. G., Ito, T. A., Finkelstein, N. D., & Pollock, S. J. (2013). How a gender gap in belonging contributes to the gender gap in physics participation. In AIP conference proceedings (Vol. 1513, No. 1, pp. 402–405).
Acknowledgements
This study was conducted as the first author’s senior thesis at the Academic Magnet High School, Charleston, SC. The authors would like to thank Donna Taylor and Judith Peterson from the Academic Magnet High School for their guidance and support throughout this project.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Hand, S., Rice, L. & Greenlee, E. Exploring teachers’ and students’ gender role bias and students’ confidence in STEM fields. Soc Psychol Educ 20, 929–945 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-017-9408-8
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-017-9408-8