Validation of a Web App Enabling Children with Dyslexia to Identify Personalized Visual and Auditory Parameters Facilitating Online Text Reading
<p>Screenshot of the presentation of the two font families (handwriting/script in the left box and serif in the right box) in the subjective procedure of the Seleggo test. The translations of the instructions in the upper part of the screen and of the text in the blue buttons as shown in the screenshot are the following: “First of all read the following passages, written with two different groups of print characters. Choose the one you feel you can read more easily”. “USA QUESTO” = “Use this one”. “AVANTI” = “forward”. The text in the white boxes is an excerpt of a history book describing Mary Stuart’s life and the textual content of the two boxes is identical except for the fonts used.</p> "> Figure 2
<p>Screenshot of the recommendations for font, size, spacing, speed and pitch as presented on the screen at the end of the first and second phase of the Seleggo test. The translations of the instructions (upper part of the screen) and of the information reported in the screenshot are the following: “Here are the parameters suggested to be used in the Seleggo reader. Click on the buttons “access” or “register” to save your data or print this page as a reminder”. “GRANDEZZA” = “size”; “SPAZIATURA” = “spacing”; “VELOCITA” = “speed”. The text in the blue buttons at the bottom of the screen can be translated as follows: “REGISTRATI” = “register”; “ACCEDI” = “access”; “STAMPA” = “print”.</p> "> Figure 3
<p>Screenshot of the examiner interface for assessment of reading accuracy after the nonword reading test (reading time is recorded automatically by the system). The instructions in the upper part of the screenshot read: “Click on the “play” button to the left of each trace in order to listen to the student’s reading. Then, indicate whether the word has been read correctly (at first try), incorrectly or self-corrected (wrong in the first try and corrected immediately afterwards, without any alert about the error).” The titles of the table shown in the screenshot are translated as follows: “Accuratezza” = “accuracy”; “Tempo di lettura” = “reading time”; “Da rileggere” = “to be read again”. The first column on the left shows the nonwords that were shown to the child; by clicking on the “play” button, the examiner can listen to the recording of the child’s reading.</p> "> Figure 4
<p>Descriptive results of font type selection outcomes are shown for the TD group (<b>a</b>) and for the AD group (<b>b</b>) separately.</p> "> Figure 5
<p>Outcomes of the objective procedure for the personalization of font size (<b>a</b>) and spacing (<b>b</b>), for the two groups (TD and AD).</p> "> Figure 6
<p>Outcomes of the objective procedure for the selection of TTS voice speed (<b>a</b>) and pitch (<b>b</b>), for the two groups (TD and AD).</p> "> Figure 7
<p>Text reading speed (syll/s): comparison of Standard and Seleggo parameters (* indicates significant differences at <span class="html-italic">p</span> < 0.05, 1-tailed).</p> "> Figure 8
<p>Text reading errors: comparison of Standard and Seleggo parameters (* indicates significant differences at <span class="html-italic">p</span> < 0.05, 1-tailed).</p> "> Figure 9
<p>Dictation errors in the whole group of children (<b>a</b>) and in TD e AD groups (<b>b</b>) separately (* indicates significant differences at <span class="html-italic">p</span> < 0.05, 1-tailed).</p> ">
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Background: Developmental Dyslexia and Perception
1.2. Dyslexia-Friendly Fonts and Text-to-Speech Technologies
1.3. The Seleggo Platform and the Aim of the Study
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Measures and Procedure
2.2.1. Procedure
2.2.2. Seleggo Test Application
- (a)
- First of all, each child reads different lists of 96 nonwords (perfectly balanced for presence of graphemes, consonants, vowels and their position within the nonword) written in different fonts and sizes (4 sizes for each of 6 different fonts resulting from subjective selection, each combination applied to 4 different nonwords). Accuracy is calculated as the percentage of reading errors (it is possible to have correct, incorrect or self-corrected reading, counting as half, i.e., 0,5 points), while speed is measured in syllables/second. Based on reading speed and errors, the software identifies optimal fonts and sizes.
- (b)
- Subsequently, these parameters are used to display 4 pseudosentences with different combinations of inter-letter and inter-line spacing. Each pseudosentence is made up of nonwords occupying three to four lines on the screen (depending on spacing parameters). Each pseudosentence is composed of 21 nonwords (2 monosyllabic, 5 bisyllabic, 7 trisyllabic, 5 quadrisyllabic, and 2 pentasyllabic nonwords) summing up to a total of 63 syllables, 39 characters and 5 punctuation symbols (3 commas and 2 full stops). All vowels are represented the same amount of times in all pseudosentences, and so are consonants, but never in the same combination. Accuracy is expressed as the percentage of words read incorrectly, while speed is reported in syll/sec. Based on reading speed and accuracy, Seleggo Test selects the best combination of font, size and spacing for each child. The selected combination is shown on the screen and the child is invited to print it or save it (see Figure 2).
- (c)
- As a last step, a series of 9 pseudosentences (made up of different non-words) appear on the PC screens and the child is asked to listen to the speech synthesis reading the pseudosentences, with different combinations of speed and pitch. For each pseudosentence, the last nonword read is not displayed on the screen, and the child is asked to choose, among four options, the one corresponding to the nonword heard. The pseudosentences are constructed so as to follow the phonotactic rules of Italian sentences and to suggest the presence of a pseudo-morphosyntactic structure that is completed by the last nonword. Each pseudosentence is composed of three nonwords, one of which is an existing Italian preposition. The second nonword is a bisyllabic nonword whose grammatical category is completely non-transparent. All final nonwords are quadrisyllabic nonwords formed by 3 CV and 1 CVC or CCV syllable, respecting the phonotactic rules of Italian, for a total amount of 9 letters. The different vowels and consonants are equally represented in the nonwords, and their combinations and positions are balanced across conditions. Each of the quadruplets for multiple choice are composed of the target nonwords, one nonword with 3 correct and 1 incorrect syllable, one with 2 correct and 2 incorrect syllables, one with 1 correct and 3 incorrect syllables. The change producing an incorrect syllable affects a single letter of the syllable, which is usually substituted by a phonologically similar element. According to the accuracy of the answer (calculated as the number of correct syllables), the best combination of speed and pitch is selected for each child. Also, in this case, the selected combination is visualized on the screen and the child is invited to either print it or save it for future use.
2.2.3. Seleggo Test Validation
2.3. Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Results of Seleggo Test
3.1.1. Visual Parameters
3.1.2. Auditory Parameters
3.2. Results of the Validation Tests
4. Discussion
5. Limitations of the Study and Future Directions
6. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- World Health Organization. International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, 10th ed.; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Barbiero, C.; Montico, M.; Lonciari, I.; Monasta, L.; Penge, R.; Vio, C.; Tressoldi, P.E.; Carrozzi, M.; De Petris, A.; De Cagno, A.G.; et al. The lost children: The underdiagnosis of dyslexia in Italy. A cross-sectional national study. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0210448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peterson, R.L.; Pennington, B.F. Developmental dyslexia. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2015, 11, 283–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ramus, F.; White, S.; Frith, U. Weighing the evidence between competing theories of dyslexia. Dev. Sci. 2006, 9, 265–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McGrath, L.M.; Peterson, R.L.; Pennington, B.F. The multiple deficit model: Progress, problems, and prospects. Sci. Stud. Read. 2020, 24, 7–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pennington, B.F. From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition 2006, 101, 385–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stein, J.; Walsh, V. To see but not to read; the magnocellular theory of dyslexia. Trends Neurosci. 1997, 20, 147–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Valdois, S.; Reilhac, C.; Ginestet, E.; Line Bosse, M. Varieties of Cognitive Profiles in Poor Readers: Evidence for a VAS-Impaired Subtype. J. Learn. Disabil. 2020, 54, 221–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Geiger, G.; Lettvin, J.Y. Dyslexia and reading as examples of alternative visual strategies. Brain Read. 1989, 34, 331–343. [Google Scholar]
- Spinelli, D.; De Luca, M.; Judica, A.; Zoccolotti, P. Crowding effects on word identification in developmental dyslexia. Cortex 2002, 38, 179–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goswami, U. A temporal sampling framework for developmental dyslexia. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011, 15, 3–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vidyasagar, T.R. Neural underpinnings of dyslexia as a disorder of visuo-spatial attention. Clin. Exp. Optom. 2004, 87, 4–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jeon, S.T.; Hamid, J.; Maurer, D.; Lewis, T.L. Developmental changes during childhood in single-letter acuity and its crowding by surrounding contours. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2010, 107, 423–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gori, S.; Facoetti, A. How the visual aspects can be crucial in reading acquisition? The intriguing case of crowding and developmental dyslexia. J. Vis. 2015, 15, 8. [Google Scholar]
- Whitney, C.; Cornelissen, P. Letter-position encoding and dyslexia. J. Res. Read. 2005, 28, 274–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bouma, H. Interaction effects in parafoveal letter recognition. Nature 1970, 226, 177–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pelli, D.G.; Tillman, K.A. Parts, Wholes, and Context in Reading: A Triple Dissociation. PLoS ONE 2007, 2, e680. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lorusso, M.L.; Facoetti, A.; Pesenti, S.; Cattaneo, C.; Molteni, M.; Geiger, G. Wider recognition in peripheral vision common to different subtypes of dyslexia. Vis. Res. 2004, 44, 2413–2424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Legge, G.E.; Bigelow, C.A. Does print size matter for reading? A review of findings from vision science and typography. J. Vis. 2011, 11, 8. [Google Scholar]
- Joo, S.J.; White, A.L.; Strodtman, D.J.; Yeatman, J.D. Optimizing text for an individual’s visual system: The contribution of visual crowding to reading difficulties. Cortex 2018, 103, 291–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hakvoort, B.; van den Boer, M.; Leenaars, T.; Bos, P.; Tijms, J. Improvements in reading accuracy as a result of increased interletter spacing are not specific to children with dyslexia. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2017, 164, 101–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zorzi, M.; Barbiero, C.; Facoetti, A.; Lonciari, I.; Carrozzi, M.; Montico, M.; Bravar, L.; George, F.; Pech-Georgel, C.; Ziegler, J.C. Extra-large letter spacing improves reading in dyslexia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2012, 109, 11455–11459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perea, M.; Panadero, V.; Moret-Tatay, C.; Gómez, P. The effects of inter-letter spacing in visual-word recognition: Evidence with young normal readers and developmental dyslexics. Learn. Instr. 2012, 22, 420–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Łuniewska, M.; Wójcik, M.; Jednoróg, K. The effect of inter-letter spacing on reading performance and eye movements in typically reading and dyslexic children. Learn. Instr. 2022, 80, 101576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rello, L.; Baeza-Yates, R. The Effect of Font Type on Screen Readability by People with Dyslexia. ACM Trans. Access. Comput. 2016, 8, 1–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rello, L.; Baeza-Yates, R. In Good fonts for dyslexia. In Proceedings of the 15th International ACM SIGACCESS Conference on Computers and Accessibility, Bellevue, WA, USA, 21–23 October 2013; pp. 1–8. [Google Scholar]
- Rello, L.; Pielot, M.; Marcos, M.; Carlini, R. Size Matters (Spacing Not): 18 Points for a Dyslexic-Friendly Wikipedia. In Proceedings of the 10th International Cross-Disciplinary Conference on Web Accessibility (W4A ’13), Rio de Janeiro, Brazil, 13–15 May 2013; ACM: New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 1–4. [Google Scholar]
- Rello, L.; Baeza-Yates, R.; Llisterri, J. A resource of errors written in Spanish by people with dyslexia and its linguistic, phonetic and visual analysis. Lang. Resour. Eval. 2017, 51, 379–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perondi, L.; Gerbino, W.; Chia, G.; Arista, R.; Pignoni, G.; Gaudenzi, G. Tipografia parametrica e developmental dyslexia. MD J. 2017, 3, 88–113. [Google Scholar]
- Bachmann, C. EasyReading™ as a compensating tool for readers with dyslexia: A comparison between Times New Roman and EasyReading™ in good readers and dyslexic fourth grade children. Dislessia G. Ital. Ric. Clin. E Appl. 2013, 10, 243–262. [Google Scholar]
- Duranovic, M.; Senka, S.; Babic-Gavric, B. Influence of increased letter spacing and font type on the reading ability of dyslexic children. Ann. Dyslexia 2018, 68, 218–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kuster, S.M.; van Weerdenburg, M.; Gompel, M.; Bosman, A.M.T. Dyslexie font does not benefit reading in children with or without dyslexia. Ann. Dyslexia 2018, 68, 25–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wery, J.J.; Diliberto, J.A. The effect of a specialized dyslexia font, OpenDyslexic, on reading rate and accuracy. Ann. Dyslexia 2017, 67, 114–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Galliussi, J.; Perondi, L.; Chia, G.; Gerbino, W.; Bernardis, P. Inter-letter spacing, inter-word spacing, and font with dyslexia-friendly features: Testing text readability in people with and without dyslexia. Ann. Dyslexia 2020, 70, 141–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Milani, A.; Lorusso, M.L.; Molteni, M. The effects of audiobooks on the psychosocial adjustment of pre-adolescents and adolescents with dyslexia. Dyslexia 2010, 16, 87–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Keelor, J.L.; Creaghead, N.A.; Silbert, N.H.; Breit, A.D.; Horowitz-Kraus, T. Impact of text-to-speech features on the reading comprehension of children with reading and language difficulties. Ann. Dyslexia 2023, 73, 469–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silvestri, R.; Holmes, A.; Rahemtulla, R. The interaction of cognitive profiles and text-to-speech software on reading comprehension of adolescents with reading challenges. J. Spec. Educ. Technol. 2022, 37, 498–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Law 170/2010 New Norms on Learning Disabilities (L.170/2010 Nuove Norme in Materia di Disturbi Specifici di Apprendimento in Ambito Scolastico). Available online: http://www.gazzettaufficiale.it/gunewsletter/dettaglio.jsp?service=1&datagu=2010-10-18&task=dettaglio&numgu=244&redaz=010G0192&tmstp=1288002517919 (accessed on 7 September 2023).
- Available online: https://www.seleggo.org/ (accessed on 7 September 2023).
- Bachmann, C.; Mengheri, L. Dyslexia and Fonts: Is a Specific Font Useful? Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lorusso, M.L.; Toraldo, A. Revisiting Multifactor Models of Dyslexia: Do they fit empirical data and what are their implications for intervention? Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
TD Children n = 29 | AD Children n = 49 | Group Comparison | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Median | Range | Median | Range | Mann–Whitney U, p | |
TTS matching accuracy (mean syllables) | 3.93 | 3.59–4 | 3.78 | 2.96–4 | 416, 0.004 |
Word reading speed (syll/s) | 1.34 | 0.73–1.67 | 1.07 | 0.47–1.56 | 390, 0.001 |
Word reading accuracy (errors) | 7.30 | 0.50–45.8 | 19.27 | 2.1–53.1 | 282.5, <0.001 |
Pseudosentence reading speed (syll/s) | 1.62 | 1–2 | 1.30 | 0–2 | 405.5, 0.002 |
Pseudosentence reading accuracy (errors) | 4.82 | 0.51–31.22 | 11.93 | 1.52–59.14 | 285, <0.001 |
Whole Sample (n = 78) | |||
---|---|---|---|
Median (Range) | Wilcoxon Test Z (p, 1-Tailed) | ||
Text reading speed (syll/s) | Seleggo | 2.85 (0.51–4.78) | −1.66 (0.049) |
Standard | 2.85 (0.54–4.70) | ||
Text reading accuracy (errors %) | Seleggo | 1.63 (0–29.32) | −1.91 (0.028) |
Standard | 2.69 (0.48–22.92) | ||
Dictation accuracy (errors) | Seleggo | 9 (1–13) | −1.65 (0.0495) |
Standard | 9 (4–15) |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Lorusso, M.L.; Borasio, F.; Panetto, P.; Curioni, M.; Brotto, G.; Pons, G.; Carsetti, A.; Molteni, M. Validation of a Web App Enabling Children with Dyslexia to Identify Personalized Visual and Auditory Parameters Facilitating Online Text Reading. Multimodal Technol. Interact. 2024, 8, 5. https://doi.org/10.3390/mti8010005
Lorusso ML, Borasio F, Panetto P, Curioni M, Brotto G, Pons G, Carsetti A, Molteni M. Validation of a Web App Enabling Children with Dyslexia to Identify Personalized Visual and Auditory Parameters Facilitating Online Text Reading. Multimodal Technologies and Interaction. 2024; 8(1):5. https://doi.org/10.3390/mti8010005
Chicago/Turabian StyleLorusso, Maria Luisa, Francesca Borasio, Paola Panetto, Mariangela Curioni, Giada Brotto, Giulio Pons, Alex Carsetti, and Massimo Molteni. 2024. "Validation of a Web App Enabling Children with Dyslexia to Identify Personalized Visual and Auditory Parameters Facilitating Online Text Reading" Multimodal Technologies and Interaction 8, no. 1: 5. https://doi.org/10.3390/mti8010005
APA StyleLorusso, M. L., Borasio, F., Panetto, P., Curioni, M., Brotto, G., Pons, G., Carsetti, A., & Molteni, M. (2024). Validation of a Web App Enabling Children with Dyslexia to Identify Personalized Visual and Auditory Parameters Facilitating Online Text Reading. Multimodal Technologies and Interaction, 8(1), 5. https://doi.org/10.3390/mti8010005