A Dynamic Interactive Approach to Music Listening: The Role of Entrainment, Attunement and Resonance
<p>Waveform (<b>upper panel</b>) and spectrogram (<b>lower panel</b>) of the first 31 s of Busoni’s transcription of the Intermezzo of Bach’s Toccata, Adagio & Fugue in C, BWV 564.</p> "> Figure 1 Cont.
<p>Waveform (<b>upper panel</b>) and spectrogram (<b>lower panel</b>) of the first 31 s of Busoni’s transcription of the Intermezzo of Bach’s Toccata, Adagio & Fugue in C, BWV 564.</p> "> Figure 2
<p>Waveform and spectrogram representation of the first four bars of the aria of Bach’s Goldberg variations (<b>upper panels</b>) and score notation of the first seven bars (<b>lower panels</b>), performed by Glenn Gould.</p> "> Figure 2 Cont.
<p>Waveform and spectrogram representation of the first four bars of the aria of Bach’s Goldberg variations (<b>upper panels</b>) and score notation of the first seven bars (<b>lower panels</b>), performed by Glenn Gould.</p> "> Figure 3
<p>Example of phase and period relationship as exemplified in a sinusoidal depiction of a periodic stimulus.</p> ">
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Music Listening: Enacting the Sounds
2.1. Eliciting Features of Entraining Stimuli
2.2. Attunement and Attention
3. Entrainment: What? What For?
3.1. Conceptual and Definitional Issues
3.2. Alignment and Adaptation
3.3. Oscillatory Dynamics: External and Internal Oscillators
4. Enactive Listening and Dynamic Interactionism
4.1. Interaction Dynamics and Mutual Entrainment
4.2. Relational Dynamics and Participatory Sense-Making
5. Conclusions and Perspectives
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Clayton, M.; Sager, R.; Wil, U. In time with the music: The concept of entrainment and its significance for Ethnomusicol. Eur. Meet. Ethnomusic. 2005, 11, 1–82. [Google Scholar]
- Sebanz, N.; Knoblich, G. Prediction in joint action: What, when, and where. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2009, 1, 353–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sebanz, N.; Bekkering, H.; Knoblich, G. Joint action: Bodies and minds moving together. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2006, 10, 70–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schneck, D.; Berger, D. The Music Effect. Music Physiology and Clinical Applications; Kingsley Publishers: Philadelphia, PA, USA; London, UK, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Keil, C. Motion and Feeling through Music. J. Aesthet. Art Crit. 1966, 24, 337–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blacking, J. (Ed.) The Anthropology of the Body; Academic Press: London, UK, 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Lomax, A. The cross-cultural variation of rhythmic style. In Interaction Rhythms. Periodicity in Human Behavior; Davis, M., Ed.; Human Sciences Press: New York, NY, USA, 1982; pp. 149–174. [Google Scholar]
- Keil, C.; Feld, S. Music Grooves: Essays and Dialogues? University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Keil, C. The Theory of Participatory Discrepancies: A Progress Report. Ethnomusicology 1995, 39, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van der Schyff, D.; Schiavio, A.; Elliott, D. Musical Bodies, Musical Minds. Enactive Cognitive Science and the Meaning of Human Musicality; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA; London, UK, 2022. [Google Scholar]
- Reybrouck, M.; Vuust, P.; Brattico, E. Neural Correlates of Music Listening: Does the Music Matter? Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 1553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Raffman, D. Language, Music and Mind; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, S. The Unification of the Arts. A Framework for Understanding What the Arts Share and Why; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2022. [Google Scholar]
- Large, E.; Snyder, J. Pulse and Meter as Neural Resonance. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2009, 1169, 46–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cooper, G.; Meyer, L.B. The Rhythmic Structure of Music; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1960. [Google Scholar]
- Reybrouck, M. Deixis in Musical Narrative Musical Sense-making between Discrete Particulars and Synoptic Overview. Chin. Semiot. Stud. 2015, 11, 79–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leman, M.; Buhmann, J.; Van Dyck, E. The empowering effects of being locked into the beat of the music. In Body, Sound and Space in Music and Beyond: Multimodal Explorations; Wöllner, C., Ed.; Routledge: London, UK, 2016; pp. 13–28. [Google Scholar]
- Vuilleumier, P.; Trost, W. Music and emotions: From enchantment to entrainment. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 2015, 1337, 212–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Keil, C. Participatory Discrepancies and the Power of Music. Cult Anthropol. 1987, 2, 275–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Large, E.W.; Jones, M.R. The dynamics of attending: How people track time-varying events. Psychol. Rev. 1999, 106, 119–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reybrouck, M. Experience as cognition: Musical sense-making and the ‘in-time/outside-of-time’ dichotomy. Int. Stud. Musicol. 2019, 19, 53–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Reybrouck, M. Musical Sense-Making. Enaction, Experience, and Computation; Routledge: Abingdon, UK; New York, NY, USA, 2021. [Google Scholar]
- Balzano, G. Music Perception as Detection of Pitch-Time Constraints. In Event Cognition: An Ecological Perspective; McCabe, V., Balzano, G., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA; London, UK, 1986; pp. 217–233. [Google Scholar]
- Lombardo, T.J. The Reciprocity of Perceiver and Environment. The Evolution of James J. Gibson’s Ecological Psychology; Lawrence Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA; London, UK, 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Wittmann, M. Moments in Time. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2011, 5, 66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Wittmann, M.; Pöppel, E. Temporal mechanisms of the brain as fundamentals of communication—With special reference to Music Percept. and performance. Music Sci. 1999, 3, 13–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Godøy, R.I. Imagined Action, Excitation, and Resonance. In Musical Imagery; Godøy, R.I., Jørgensen, H., Eds.; Swets & Zeitlinger: Lisse, The Netherlands, 2001; pp. 237–250. [Google Scholar]
- Reybrouck, M. A Biosemiotic and Ecological Approach to Music Cognition: Event Perception between Auditory Listening and Cognitive Economy. Axiomathes 2005, 15, 229–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- James, W. Psychology: The Briefer Course; Harper & Row: New York, NY, USA, 1961. [Google Scholar]
- Jones, M. Time Will Tell: A Theory of Dynamic Attending, Online Edition; Oxford Academic: New York, NY, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Jones, M.R.; Bolz, M. Dynamic attending and responding to time. Psychol. Rev. 1989, 96, 459–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Volgsten, U. The roots of music: Emotional expression, dialogue and affect attunement in the psychogenesis of music. Music Sci. 2012, 16, 200–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holck, U.; Geretsegger, M. Musical and emotional attunement: Unique and essential in music therapy with children on the autism spectrum. Nord. J. Music Ther. 2016, 25, 34–35. [Google Scholar]
- Gabrielsson, A. Timing in music performance and its relations to music experience. In Generative Processes in Music: The Psychology of Performance, Improvisation, and Composition; Sloboda, J., Ed.; Clarendon Press: Oxford, UK, 1988; pp. 27–51. [Google Scholar]
- Palmer, C. Mapping musical thought to musical performance. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1989, 15, 331–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Repp, B.H.; Iversen, J.R.; Patel, A.D. Tracking an imposed beat within a metrical grid. Music Percept. 2008, 26, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jones, M.R. Time, Our Lost Dimension: Toward a New Theory of Perception, Attention, and Memory. Psychol. Rev. 1976, 83, 323–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Drake, C.; Jones, M.R.; Baruch, J. The development of rhythmic attending in auditory sequences: Attunement, referent period, focal attending. Cognition 2000, 77, 251–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Clarke, E. Timers, oscillators and entrainment. Eur. Meet. Ethnomusic. 2005, 11, 49–50. [Google Scholar]
- Seemann, A. The Other Person in Joint Attention. A Relational Approach. J. Conscious. Stud. 2010, 17, 161–182. [Google Scholar]
- James, W. The Principles of Psychology, I; Holt and Company: New York, NY, USA, 1890. [Google Scholar]
- Theeuwes, J. Endogenous and exogenous control of visual selection. Perception 1994, 23, 429–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Theeuwes, J. Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture. Psychon Bull Rev. 2004, 11, 65–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Berlyne, D.E. Attention. In Handbook of Perception: Historical and Philosophical Roots of Perception; Carterette, E.C., Friedman, P.P., Eds.; Harcourt Brace Jovanovich: New York, NY, USA, 1974; Volume 1, pp. 123–147. [Google Scholar]
- Neumann, O. Theories of attention. In Handbook of Perception and Action. III: Attention; Neumann, O., Sanders, A.F., Eds.; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 1996; pp. 389–446. [Google Scholar]
- Posner, M. Orienting of attention. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1980, 32, 3–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sokolov, E.N. The neuronal mechanisms of the orienting reflex. In Neuronal Mechanisms of the Orienting Reflex; Sokolov, E.N., Vinogradova, O.S., Eds.; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1975; pp. 217–338. [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt, R.; Carello, C.; Turvey, M. Phase Transitions and Critical Fluctuations in the Visual Coordination of Rhythmic Movements Between People. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1990, 16, 227–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Reybrouck, M. Music Shaped in Time: Musical Sense-making between Perceptual Immediacy and Symbolic Representation. Rech. Sémiotiques 2016, 36, 99–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Stewart, M.L. The Feel Factor: Music with Soul. 1987. Available online: https://www.mcavinchey.org/music-article/8363/details (accessed on 16 April 2023).
- Prögler, J. Searching for Swing: Participatory Discrepancies in the Jazz Rhythm Section. Ethnomusicology 1995, 39, 21–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clayton, M. What is Entrainment? Definition and applications in musical research. Empir. Musicol. Rev. 2012, 7, 49–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pantaleone, J. Synchronization of metronomes. Am. J. Phys. 2002, 70, 992–1000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wiesenfeld, K. Huygens’s Odd Sympathy Recreated. Societ. Polit. 2017, 11, 15–22. [Google Scholar]
- Geng, Y.; Liu, S.; Yin, Z.; Naik, A.; Prabhakar, B.; Rosenblum, M. Exploiting a Natural Network Effect for Scalable, Fine-grained Clock Synchronization. In Proceedings of the 15th USENIX Symposium on Networked Systems Design and Implementation, Renton, WA, USA, 9–11 April 2018; Usenix: Renton, WA, USA, 2018; pp. 81–94. [Google Scholar]
- Bennett, M.; Schatz, M.; Rockwood, H.; Wiesenfeld, K. Huygens’s clocks. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. 2002, A 458, 563–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thaut, M.; McIntosh, G.; Hoemberg, V. Neurobiological Foundations of neurologic music therapy: Rhythmic entrainment and the motor system. Front. Psychol. 2015, 5, 1185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schmidt, R.; Turvey, M. Phase-entrainment dynamics of visually coupled rhythmic movements. Biol. Cyber. 1994, 70, 369–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roenneberg, T.; Hut, R.; Daan, S.; Merrow, M. Entrainment Concepts Revisited. J. Biol. Rhythm. 2010, 25, 329–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Allgayer-Kaufmann, R. Identification the Entrainment Process by the Degree of Synchronization. Eur. Meet. Ethnomusicol. 2005, 11, 46–47. [Google Scholar]
- Chang, A.; Bosnyak, D.; Trainor, L. Unpredicted Pitch Modulates Beta Oscillatory Power during Rhythmic Entrainment to a Tone Sequence. Front Psychol. 2018, 7, 327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Secora Pearl, J. Cognitive vs. physical entrainment. Eur. Meet. Ethnomusic. 2005, 11, 61–63. [Google Scholar]
- Reybrouck, M.; Eerola, T. Music and its inductive power: A psychobiological and evolutionary approach to musical emotions. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Rayleigh, J. The Theory of Sound; Dover Publishers: New York, NY, USA, 1945. [Google Scholar]
- Winfree, A. When Time Breaks Down: The Three-Dimensional Dynamics of Electrochemical Waves and Cardiac Arrhythmias; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Stepp, N.; Turvey, M. On Strong Anticipation. Cogn. Syst. Res. 2010, 11, 148–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Peña Ramirez, P.J.; Fey, R.H.B.; Nijmeijer, H. Synchronization of weakly nonlinear oscillators with Huygens’ coupling. Chaos 2013, 23, 033118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Peña Ramirez, J.P.; Olvera, L.A.; Nijmeijer, H.; Alvarez, J. The sympathy of two pendulum clocks: Beyond Huygens’ observations. Sci. Rep. 2016, 6, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Peña Ramirez, J.P.; Henk Nijmeijer, H. The Poincaré method: A powerful tool for analyzing synchronization of coupled oscillators. Indag. Math. 2016, 27, 1127–1146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Strogatz, S.H.; Stewart, I. Coupled oscillators and biological synchronization. Sci. Am. 1993, 269, 68–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gordon, C.; Cobb, P.; Balasubramaniam, R. Recruitment of the motor system during music listening: An ALE meta-analysis of fMRI data. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0207213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Prinz, W.; Chater, N. An Ideomotor Approach to Imitation. In Perspectives on Imitation: From Neuroscience to Social Science: Mechanisms of Imitation and Imitation in Animals; Hurley, S., Ed.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2005; Volume 1, pp. 141–156. [Google Scholar]
- Reybrouck, M. Musical Imagery between Sensory Processing and Ideomotor Simulation. In Musical Imagery; Godøy, R.I., Jörgensen, H., Eds.; Swets & Zeitlinger: Lisse, The Netherlands, 2001; pp. 117–136. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, S.; Merker, B.; Wallin, N. An introduction to evolutionary musicology. In The Origins of Music; Wallin, N., Merker, B., Brown, S., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2000; pp. 3–24. [Google Scholar]
- Buhrmann, T.; Di Paolo, E.; Barandiaran, X. A Dynamical Systems Account of Sensorimotor Contingencies. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Di Paolo, E.A.; Buhrmann, T.; Barandiaran, X.E. Sensorimotor Life: An Enactive Proposal; Oxford UP: New York, NY, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Di Paolo, E.; Barandiaran, X.; Beaton, M.; Buhrmann, T. Learning to perceive in the sensorimotor approach: Piaget’s theory of equilibration interpreted dynamically. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Repp, B.; Yi-Huang Su, Y. Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of recent research (2006–2012). Psychon. Bull Rev. 2013, 20, 403–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- O’Regan, K.; Noë, A. What is it like to see: A sensorimotor theory of perceptual experience. Synthese 2002, 129, 79–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Todd, N.; Lee, S. The sensory-motor theory of rhythm and beat induction 20 years on: A new synthesis and futures perspective. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Torrance, S. The skill of seeing: Beyond the sensorimotor account? Trends Cogn. Sci. 2002, 6, 495–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Silverman, D. Sensorimotor enactivism and temporal experience. Adapt. Behav. 2013, 21, 151–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Koelsch, S.; Vuust, P.; Friston, K. Predictive Processes and the Peculiar Case of Music. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2019, 23, 63–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Vuust, P.; Witek, M.A. Rhythmic complexity and predictive coding: A novel approach to modeling rhythm and meter perception in music. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 1111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Williams, D. Predictive Processing and the Representation Wars. Minds Mach. 2018, 28, 141–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Friston, K. A theory of cortical responses. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2005, 360, 815–836. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Galantucci, B.; Fowler, C.A.; Turvey, M.T. The motor theory of speech perception reviewed. Psychon. Bull Rev. 2006, 3, 361–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sherwood, D.; Lee, T. Schema Theory: Critical Review and Implications for the Role of Cognition in a New Theory of Motor Learning. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 2003, 74, 376–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vihman, M.M. Ontogeny of phonetic gestures: Speech production. In Modularity and the Motor Theory of Speech Perception; Mattingly, I., Studdert-Kennedy, M., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1991; pp. 69–84. [Google Scholar]
- Magill, R.A.; Anderson, D.I. (Eds.) Motor Learning and Control: Concepts and Applications; McGraw Hill: New York, NY, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt, R.A. A schema theory of discrete motor learning. Psychol. Rev. 1975, 82, 225–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moens, B.; Leman, M. Alignment strategies for the entrainment of music and movement rhythms. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2015, 1, 86–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Richardson, M.J.; Marsh, K.L.; Schmidt, R.C. Effects of visual and verbal interaction on unintentional interpersonal coordination. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2005, 31, 62–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Schmidt, R.C.; Richardson, M.J. Dynamics of interpersonal coordination. In Coordination: Neural, Behavioral and Social Dynamics; Fuchs, A., Ed.; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 2008; pp. 281–308. [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch, W. An algorithm for the EEG frequency architecture of consciousness and brain body coupling. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Klimesch, W. The frequency architecture of brain and brain body oscillations: An analysis. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2018, 48, 2431–2453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Canavier, C.C. Phase-resetting as a tool of information transmission. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2015, 31, 206–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Herrmann, B.; Henry, M.; Haegens, S.; Obleser, J. Temporal expectations and neural amplitude fluctuations in auditory cortex interactively influence perception. NeuroImage 2016, 124, 487–497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Palmer, C.; Krumhansl, C.L. Mental representations for musical meter. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1990, 16, 728–741. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arnal, L.; Doelling, K.B.; Poeppel, D. Delta–Beta Coupled Oscillations Underlie Temporal Prediction Accuracy. Cereb Cortex. 2015, 25, 3077–3085. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fujioka, T.; Trainor, L.; Large, E.; Ross, B. Beta and Gamma Rhythms in Human Auditory Cortex during Musical Beat Processing. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2009, 1169, 89–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Large, E.W. Resonating to musical rhythm: Theory and experiment. In The Psychology of Time; Grondin, S., Ed.; Emerald: Bingley, UK, 2008; pp. 189–231. [Google Scholar]
- Nobre, A.C.; van Ede, F. Anticipated moments: Temporal structure in attention. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2018, 19, 34–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Andersen, L.; Dalal, S. The cerebellar clock: Predicting and timing somatosensory touch. NeuroImage 2021, 238, 118202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosso, M.; Heggli, O.; Maes, P.J.; Vuust, P.; Leman, M. Mutual beta power modulation in dyadic entrainment. NeuroImage 2022, 257, 119326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Betti, V.; Della Penna, S.; de Pasquale, F.; Corbetta, M. Spontaneous beta band rhythms in the predictive coding of natural stimuli. Neuroscientist 2021, 27, 184–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lee, J.H.; Whittington, M.A.; Kopell, N.J. Top-down beta rhythms support selective attention via interlaminar interaction: A model. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2013, 9, e1003164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- van Ede, F.; De Lange, F.; Jensen, O.; Maris, E. Orienting attention to an upcoming tactile event involves a spatially and temporally specific modulation of sensorimotor alpha-and beta-band oscillations. J. Neurosci. 2011, 31, 2016–2024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- van Pelt, S.; Heil, L.; Kwisthout, J.; Ondobaka, S.; van Rooij, I. Bekkering Beta- and gamma-band activity reflect predictive coding in the processing of causal events. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2016, 11, 973–980. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- De Pasquale, F.; Della Penna, S.; Sporns, O.; Romani, G.L.; Corbetta, M. A dynamic core network and global efficiency in the resting human brain. Cereb. Cortex 2016, 26, 4015–4033. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- De Pasquale, F.; Corbetta, M.; Betti, V.; Della Penna, S. Cortical cores in network dynamics. Neuroimage 2018, 180, 370–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Glass, L. Synchronization and rhythmic processes in physiology. Nature 2001, 410, 277–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Colombetti, G. The Feeling Body: Affective Science Meets the Enactive Mind; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Varela, F. Principles of Biological Autonomy; North Holland: New York, NY, USA; Oxford, UK, 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Varela, F.; Thompson, E.; Rosch, E. The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Thompson, E. Life and mind: From autopoiesis to neurophenomenology. A tribute to Francisco Varela. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 2004, 3, 381–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thompson, E. Mind in Life: Biology, Phenomenology, and the Sciences of Mind; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Hanna, R.; Thompson, E. The mind-body-body problem. Theor. Hist. Sci. 2001, 7, 24–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Torrance, S. In search of the enactive: Introduction to special issue on enactive experience. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 2006, 4, 357–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thompson, E. Sensorimotor subjectivity and the enactive approach to experience. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 2006, 4, 407–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- De Jaegher, H.; Di Paolo, E.; Gallagher, S. Can social interaction constitute social cognition? Trends Cogn. Sci. 2010, 14, 441–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- De Jaegher, H.; Di Paolo, E. Enactivism is not interactionism. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 6, 345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Overgaard, S.; Michael, J. The interactive turn in social cognition research: A critique. Philos. Psychol. 2015, 28, 160–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fiebich, A.; Gallagher, S. Joint Attention in Joint Action. Philos. Psychol. 2013, 26, 571–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fuchs, T.; De Jaegher, H. Enactive intersubjectivity: Participatory sense-making and mutual incorporation. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 2009, 8, 465–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hutto, D.D. The limits of spectatorial folk psychology. Mind Lang. 2004, 19, 548–573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ratcliffe, M. Rethinking Commonsense Psychology: A Critique of Folk Psychology, Theory of Mind and Simulation; Palgrave Macmillan: Basingstoke, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Gallagher, S. Inference or interaction: Social cognition without precursors. Philos. Explor. 2008, 11, 163–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Murray, L.; Trevarthen, C. Emotional regulations of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In Social Perception in Infants; Fieldand, T.M., Fox, N.A., Eds.; Ablex: Norwood, MA, USA, 1985; pp. 177–197. [Google Scholar]
- Auvry, M.; Lenay, C.; Stewart, J. Perceptual interactions in a minimalist virtual environment. New Ideas Psychol. 2009, 27, 32–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Auvry, M.; Rohde, M. Perceptual crossing: The simplest online paradigm. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012, 6, 181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- De Jaegher, H.; Di Paolo, E. Participatory sense-making. An enactive approach to social cognition. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 2007, 6, 485–507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reybrouck, M.; Podlipniak, P.; Welch, D. Music Listening and Homeostatic Regulation: Surviving and Flourishing in a Sonic World. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bannister, S. A survey into the experience of musically induced chills: Emotions, situations and music. Psychol. Music 2020, 48, 297–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Grewe, O.; Kopiez, R.; Altenmüller, E. The Chill Parameter: Goose Bumps and Shivers as promising Measures in Emotion Research. Music Percept. 2009, 27, 61–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harrison, L.; Loui, P. Thrills, chills, frissons, and skin orgasms: Toward an integrative model of transcendent psychophysiological experiences in music. Front Psychol. 2014, 5, 790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nusbaum, E. Listening Between the Notes: Aesthetic Chills in Everyday Music Listening. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 2014, 8, 104–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Vuoskoski, J.; Eerola, T. The Pleasure Evoked by Sad Music is Mediated by Feelings of Being Moved. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wassiliwizky, E.; Wagner, V.; Jacobsen, T.; Menninghaus, W. Art-elicited chills indicate states of being moved. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 2015, 9, 405–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Di Paolo, E. Behavioral Coordination, Structural Congruence and Entrainment in a Simulation of Acoustically Coupled Agents. Adapt. Behav. 2000, 8, 27–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Merleau-Ponty, M. The child’s relations with others. In The Primacy of Perception; Edie, J.M., Ed.; Northwestern University Press: Evanston, IL, USA, 1964; pp. 96–158. [Google Scholar]
- Kojima, H.; Froese, T.; Oka, M.; Iizuka, H.; Ikegami, T. A Sensorimotor Signature of the Transition to Conscious Social Perception: Co-regulation of Active and Passive Touch. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1778. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Miyahara, K. Perceiving other agents: Passive experience for seeing the other body as the other’s body. Annu. Rev. Phenomenol. Assoc. Jpn. 2015, 31, 23–32. [Google Scholar]
- Torrance, S.; Froese, T. An Inter-Enactive Approach to Agency: Participatory Sense-Making, Dynamics, and Sociality. Hum. Mente 2011, 15, 21–53. [Google Scholar]
- Pearce, E.; Launay, J.; MacCarron, P.; Dunbar, R. Tuning in to others: Exploring relational and collective bonding in singing and non-singing groups over time. Psychol. Music 2017, 45, 496–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fodor, J.A. Methodological solipsism considered as a research strategy in cognitive psychology. Behav. Brain Sci. 1980, 3, 63–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Durkheim, E. The Elementary Forms of Religious Life; Free Press: New York, NY, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Boyd, B. On the Origin of Stories: Evolution, Cognition and Fiction; Belknap Press of Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Jordania, J. Who Asked the First Question? Origins of Choral Singing. Intelligence, Language and Speech; Logos: Tbilisi, Georgia, 2006. [Google Scholar]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Reybrouck, M. A Dynamic Interactive Approach to Music Listening: The Role of Entrainment, Attunement and Resonance. Multimodal Technol. Interact. 2023, 7, 66. https://doi.org/10.3390/mti7070066
Reybrouck M. A Dynamic Interactive Approach to Music Listening: The Role of Entrainment, Attunement and Resonance. Multimodal Technologies and Interaction. 2023; 7(7):66. https://doi.org/10.3390/mti7070066
Chicago/Turabian StyleReybrouck, Mark. 2023. "A Dynamic Interactive Approach to Music Listening: The Role of Entrainment, Attunement and Resonance" Multimodal Technologies and Interaction 7, no. 7: 66. https://doi.org/10.3390/mti7070066
APA StyleReybrouck, M. (2023). A Dynamic Interactive Approach to Music Listening: The Role of Entrainment, Attunement and Resonance. Multimodal Technologies and Interaction, 7(7), 66. https://doi.org/10.3390/mti7070066