Influence of Adaptive Human–Machine Interface on Electric-Vehicle Range-Anxiety Mitigation
<p>Vedecom (Partner from the H2020 ADAS&ME project: <a href="http://www.vedecom.fr" target="_blank">http://www.vedecom.fr</a>) Wizard of Oz (WoZ) system. (<b>left</b>) Front-right door and system cover. Interviewer slides their right arm into that cover. (<b>right</b>) Hidden joystick inside that cover. This WoZ system does not require an additional control mechanism like dual pedals.</p> "> Figure 2
<p>Navigation view of driving session. The whole session lasted 58 min for 29.5 km. Vedecom Hall A was the staring point; driver passed sequentially from points A, B, and C before the emergency stop.</p> "> Figure 3
<p>(left to right on upper line) (i) Isochrone maps that were reachable by using either normal driving or by activating eco-driving mode. (ii) The multiple paths leading to the destination, with individual estimation of the battery level at arrival. (iii) Traffic light coping strategy pop-up. (iv) Regenerative breaking indicator pop-up (left to right on lower line). (i) Icon to indicate the available charging stations. (ii) The Human–Machine Interface (HMI) proposed to go to the closest charging station. (iii) Remaining battery level too critical; therefore, critical range protection launched. (iv) Call assistance and call taxi proposals in battery breakdown.</p> "> Figure 4
<p>Human–Machine Interface (HMI) note-taker is an in-vehicle live annotation tool. This tool was used to pilot the main HMI and a annotate perceived driver state.</p> "> Figure 5
<p>Test vehicle and adaptive HMI on mounted tablet PC.</p> "> Figure 6
<p>Twenty-two users rated how these personal traits applied to them according to Ten Item Personality Measure (TIPI) scale: strongly disagree (1), moderately disagree (2), disagree a little (3), neither agree nor disagree (4), agree a little (5), moderately agree (6), and strongly agree (7).</p> "> Figure 7
<p>Comparison of TIPI results from user group with 1813 people (this study, blue; norms, red). Z-scores per personality inventory: extroversion, 0.607; agreeableness, 0.115; conscientiousness, 0.455; emotional stability, 0.112; openness, 0.467.</p> "> Figure 8
<p>Mean values for responses to the following questions at the beginning of the experiment (from twenty-one participants): (1) Do you feel angry? (2) Do you feel relaxed with what we are going to do? (3) Do you feel irritable? (4) Do you feel confident? (5) Do you feel comfortable? (6) Today, did you get nervous about something that happened suddenly? (<math display="inline"><semantics> <mrow> <mo>−</mo> <mn>2</mn> </mrow> </semantics></math> for strongly disagree, <math display="inline"><semantics> <mrow> <mo>−</mo> <mn>1</mn> </mrow> </semantics></math> for disagree, 0 for neutral, 1 for agree, and 2 for strongly agree) (see <a href="#app2-mti-04-00004" class="html-app">Appendix B</a>).</p> "> Figure 9
<p>Mean values for responses of the following question, from twenty-one participants at the beginning of the experiment (in blue), for eleven test-group drivers at the emergency stop (red), and three control-group drivers in emergency stop (gray): (1) Do you feel relaxed? (2) Do you feel irritable? (3) Do you feel confident? (4) Do you feel nervous? (<math display="inline"><semantics> <mrow> <mo>−</mo> <mn>2</mn> </mrow> </semantics></math> for strongly disagree, <math display="inline"><semantics> <mrow> <mo>−</mo> <mn>1</mn> </mrow> </semantics></math> for disagree, 0 for neutral, 1 for agree, and 2 for strongly agree) (see <a href="#app2-mti-04-00004" class="html-app">Appendix B</a>).</p> "> Figure 10
<p>Mean values of AttrakDiff questionnaire responses, given by fifteen test-group (blue) and three control-group (red) participants (see <a href="#app2-mti-04-00004" class="html-app">Appendix B</a>).</p> ">
Abstract
:1. Introduction
- worry of being immobilized due to low vehicle battery in the future or present,
- what happens if such a situation emerges,
- not being able to find a solution to the situation, and
- being stranded in an uncomfortable situation.
- battery capacity (kWh),
- energy consumption (mostly affected by car weight; kWh/km),
- charging rate (kW), and
- minimal state of charge (%).
2. Literature Review
2.1. Range-Anxiety Expression
- Cognitive level: negative cognition associated with range, like concerns about running out of energy and not being able to reach the destination.
- Emotional level: changes in effect associated with a range situation, like feeling of nervousness or even fear.
- Behavioral level decreasing immediate anxiety by increasing perceptions of safety and control [6,7], i.e., certain activities like tapping with fingers on the steering wheel, changing driving style to save energy, or frequently checking relevant displays, e.g., range and navigation display or yelling, honking, and aggressive gesturing [8].
2.2. Range-Anxiety Mitigation Methods
3. Method
3.1. User Panel
3.2. Ethics
3.3. Expert Panel
- Technician: Mainly in charge of the preparation of the test vehicle, before the arrival of each participant and after the end of the test. These tasks ensure that the real autonomy of the EV is enough for the test, calibrating sensors and transferring the collected data to a server.
- Interviewer (placed in front-right seat): The main interlocutor of the driver. They are in charge of welcoming the participant and executing the questionnaires explained in Section 5. The expert practises active listening in a semistructured interview [24,25], and think-aloud methods used in usability testing [26].
- Observer (placed in rear-left seat): In charge of noting every relevant phrase (called a verbatim) pronounced by the driver with their observations and reactions of the driver as a human-factor specialist [27]. These were reviewed after the experiment with the aim of adjusting annotations and having better feedback.
- Annotator (placed in rear-right seat): In charge of using the HMI note-taker application (see Section 4.1) that creates annotations. The annotator is also in charge of estimating the driver state and accordingly piloting the HMI through this application (detailed in Section 4.1). The final goal of the project is implementing an automated driver-state estimator that pilots the main HMI.
3.4. Test Protocol
Protocol Phases and Theoretical Driver State
- 0
- Vehicle preparation—Before User Arrival: The technician ensured that the vehicle was ready to perform the next test. The mood of the user could hardly be evaluated. Most people are expected to wonder about what would happen in the next hour, and the feeling of uncertainty could have a negative or positive impact.
- 1
- Welcoming: At the participant’s arrival, their written consent is taken. The participant was invited to sign an audiovisual-recording agreement (for the data collection), a nondisclosure agreement, and a declaration of the validity of their driving license. Then, the participant was quickly briefed on the test without making them understand the real purpose of the test (detailed in Section 5). The interviewer had been trained to reassure the user at their arrival to be in a positive emotional state. The most important point was to let them understand that the test was easily doable by anyone.
- 2
- Interview: The following step was constructed with an interview with basic questions about their driving habits, followed by the Ten Item Personality Inventory (TIPI) questionnaire, which is a questionnaire to evaluate the participant’s personal traits [28]. Then, the interview continued with the following questions to evaluate the state of mind of the participant on the basis of the Likert scale (see Section 5.1):
- Today, did you get angry about something that happened suddenly?
- Do you feel relaxed?
- Do you feel irritable?
- Do you feel confident?
- Do you feel comfortable with what we are going to do?
- Do you feel nervous?
- 3
- Sensor Equipment: The sensor equipment is quite uncommon. As users were asked to wear a headset microphone and a biophysiological signal belt on their skin (detailed in Section 4.2), it could make the user uncomfortable at the beginning.
- 4
- Static Car Discovery: Explaining to the participant the in-vehicle sensors that collect the data mentioned in their written consent (see Welcoming phase). Simultaneously, sensors were calibrated for data acquisition by the technician (see Section 4.2).
- 5
- Dynamic Car Discovery (Positive Scenario): On a private test track, users were invited to drive the EV freely so that they familiarize themselves with it. Afterwards, they were invited to activate a fully autonomous driving mode, always on the private test track. This fully autonomous driving mode was nonexistent and simply simulated by a WoZ system. The WoZ technology is used in experiments in which participants think that the system is operating autonomously but in reality, the autonomous feature is operated by a human [31,32]. In our case, a hidden joystick located in the front-right door, used by the interviewer (see Figure 1). This part was important to avoid learning bias during scenario-data recording by letting the new participant become familiar with the unknown technologies of autonomous driving and EV, which is judged important as safety measures before going on open roads. Users were expected to be excited to use and better understand the technology.
- 6
- Positive Phase: While the vehicle continues to run in full autonomous mode, to establish a relaxed conversation, participants were asked to recall their best, happiest, and most incredible memories. Nothing from the discussion was collected. The interest of this phase was to induce a positive mood to have the highest valance as possible with the anxiety phase. These steps were designed to induce a positive emotional state to the participant.
- 7
- Neutral Driving: After the previous scenario, users were asked to take the control of the vehicle and go on open roads. This was the phase when users started to drive manually the EV. In neutral driving, participants only received indications related to which direction to take. The interviewer encouraged classical driving situations to provide a neutral reference for data acquisition, which needed neutral data for future exploitation.
- 8
- Scenario Implementations: The application of the following scenarios to real-life needed the knowledge and use of local infrastructures. Therefore, we designed a circuit where the user experimented with the presented successive scenarios (shown in Figure 2). The scenarios lasted for one hour and covered nearly 30 km. The duration of the test was specifically chosen to fit a fake autonomy decrease of 1% per minute. We set the fake autonomy to 60%. These scenarios were designed to induce three steps of anxiety. The objective was to generate as much anxiety as possible without compromising road security.
- Scenario A: This scenario lasted 20 min. The participant received no range alert, only notifications (detailed in Section 3.5) to familiarise themselves with the use of an EV and the HMI itself. The aim of this first scenario was to let the user familiarise themselves with the new EV experience by paying attention to the low noise of the car and its low autonomy. The interviewer reassures the user about the autonomy by showing tips from the HMI and how to improve battery-level management. On the basis of environmental status, the annotator launched the traffic light pop-up when the driver approached a red traffic light, and the hill pop-up when the vehicle was downhill. The estimated- and real-consumption graph was also presented while waiting on a red light. The interviewer switched from scenario A to the B when fake autonomy reached 40%.
- Scenario B: In this phase, the remaining range started to decay, therefore the user was encouraged to find a charging station and charge the EV. This scenario started below 40% of fake range remaining, and after 20 min of driving. The interviewer had to let the user understand that they were losing too much battery, and it would be better to find a charging station to refill for at least 10 min. Even if this were not critical, the driver was still encouraged to find a charging station to charge the EV. They, then, chose one of the closest charging stations, proposed by the HMI. We knew that, at that location, the closest proposed charging station was temporarily out of service. We still let the driver go to that charging station. Once they understood that the charging station was out of service, they were encouraged to choose another charging station, which was naturally the second closest charging station. The second one was available, but the driver had been provided a charging cable that was not compatible with that charging infrastructure. The interviewer and annotators acted surprised and they argued between themselves about whose fault it was for putting the wrong cable type in the trunk. To conclude this phase, the driver was told to continue the experiment, as the remaining range was enough for the rest.
- Scenario C: At this stage of the experiment, the remaining range was critical, and safety notifications were sent to the participant (see Section 3.5). Moreover, the participant has to deal with complex situations, such as taking the highway while having low remaining range. This scenario began after 40 min of testing, with ~20% fake battery remaining. After the incapacity to charge up the vehicle, users were expected to realize that battery breakdown was close, and to be anxious and lose their natural casualness with the interviewer (reduction of confidence on speech flow and driving skills). In this phase, the interviewer would become even more vigilant to ensure the safety of the test. As the fake range autonomy still decreased by 1% per minute, the last 10 min would be stressful for both user and interviewer. The interviewer had to play their role as an actor to not let the driver understand that autonomy was faked.
- 9
- Emergency Stop: After the range incident happened during Scenario C, the remaining range would be too critical. The vehicle had to be stopped at a safe spot. The HMI was imposed to take control of the vehicle in order to find that safe spot and complete the emergency stop. We could not implement an autonomous safety parking procedure, as using the WoZ outside of a private area could be dangerous. When 1% remaining battery level was reached, the interviewer became very directive, and asked the user to act on their warnings and park at the closest place.
- 10
- Discussion: Once the vehicle was immobilized, the user discovered additional capabilities of the HMI (detailed in Section 3.5). Finally, the interviewer proceeded with the same questionnaire to evaluate the state of mind of the participant, again based on the Likert scale (see Section 5.3.1):
- Do you feel relaxed?
- Do you feel irritable?
- Do you feel confident?
- Do you feel nervous?
- 11
- Revealing the purpose: Finally, the user was told about the underlying purposes of this test. Among the perceived range anxiety level, user-perceived usability (i.e., pragmatic attributes), hedonic attributes (e.g., stimulation and identification), goodness (i.e., satisfaction), and the beauty of our HMI design were interesting features to explore. To evaluate these points, participants were asked to rate the experiment and the HMI itself on the basis of some questions from the AttrakDiff scale [33,34] (detailed in Section 5.3.3). This is an important step to remove all fake knowledge induced from the user before leaving them. By being safe and confident, the user could have a proper evaluation of the real purpose of the experiment.
3.5. Adopted Coping Strategies
- Estimated battery charge at arrival: After selecting the destination, and while reviewing alternative routes, the estimated battery charge at arrival was presented for each route to reassure drivers that battery charge is sufficient (see Figure 3, upper middle-left).
- Far destination: After selecting the destination, if the battery was borderline sufficient or insufficient for reaching it, a warning was displayed with a suggestion to instead find a charging station first.
- Navigation overview: The HMI provided a navigation overview with expected traffic conditions along the route to indicate that they were taken into account in battery-consumption estimation.
- Charging stations along the route: Charging stations along the route were clearly marked on the map to assume the driver that even if any battery issues arose, they could easily be solved.
- Approaching red traffic light: When the vehicle reached a red traffic light where it needed to stop, the HMI informed the driver to gradually slow down to recover energy (see Figure 3, upper middle-right).
- Approaching a hill: When reaching a hill, the driver was informed that the battery would discharge on the way up and recharge on the way down.
- Regenerative breaking: Whenever the battery was recharged, this was communicated to the driver through a visual cue within the HMI (see Figure 3, upper right).
- Estimated and real consumption graph: When the participant began to be anxious, a graph of the battery level was displayed on the HMI. On this coping strategy, we showed the overall consumption from the start and the estimation to the destination. Thus, there was no reason for the participant to worry about the range.
- Battery warning level 1: When the battery level dropped below 40% (first threshold), the driver was prompted to save energy by using the eco-driving mode of the vehicle, or even switching to automated driving.
- Search the closest charging spots icon: An icon appears on the screen to list the losest charging stations (see Figure 3, lower left).
- Battery warning level 2: When the battery level dropped below 30% (second threshold), the HMI pre-emptively introduced an option for searching for charging stations. When selected, this option displayed a list of close charging stations, with their locations highlighted on the map.
- Battery insufficient: When the battery level became insufficient, a more direct prompt to go to the closest charging station was displayed (see Figure 3, lower middle-left).
- Book charging station: The driver could directly book a charging station from the HMI and proceed there, at which point the HMI would suggest nearby points of interest (e.g., shopping mall) that the driver could visit while waiting for the charge.
- Battery critical: If all suggestions and warnings about the declining battery level were ignored and battery level became critical, the HMI communicated to the driver the intention of the vehicle to take over soon to initiate a safety parking procedure.
- Safety-parking procedure: With 5 km left on available range, the automation took over, with the HMI instructing the driver to let go of the steering wheel and pedals (see Figure 3, lower middle-right).
- Repatriation procedure: When automation took over to drive to the closest available parking spot, a repatriation procedure for the vehicle was also automatically initiated.
- Emergency assistance: The HMI enabled directly calling a taxi or for for emergency assistance, with the driver being informed in real-time about their estimated time or arrival (see Figure 3, lower right).
3.6. Control Group
4. Experiment Implementation
4.1. Expert Ratings and Driver-State Estimation
4.2. Additional Material
- Vehicle Controller Area Network (CAN Bus): Contained numerous features for detecting the driver’s behavioral level of range anxiety, such as the vehicle’s actual speed (compared to the allowed speed) or the occurrence of hard brakes as a safety-measure metric.
- Near-infrared cameras: Two near-infrared face-tracking cameras were installed for facial expressions and gaze analysis. On the one hand, facial expression analysis provides an estimation of the emotional and anxiety level of the driver. On the other hand, the number of glances per minute to the battery indicator confirmed the source of the detected anxiety as the remaining range of the vehicle from a behavioral level.
- Shotgun microphones: Installed in front of the windshield, creating a microphone antenna. This antenna was useful to estimate the emotional anxiety level of the driver by speech analysis.
- Headset microphone: Visible through the central mirror, it was installed on the driver’s face. This sensor was useful to provide a ground-truth audio recording for the microphone antenna.
- Frequency Modulated Continuous Wave (FMCW) radar: A noninvasive solution for mean heart rate and respiration rate estimation proposed in the ADAS&ME project. These data were useful for the physiological level of anxiety estimation based on biophysiological signals. The radar was mounted inside the steering wheel and was pointed at the driver’s torso.
- Zephyr Bioharness 3 Belt (https://www.zephyranywhere.com/): This sensor was another ground-truth sensor for the FMCW radar.
- Webcams: Two webcams were also mounted on the vehicle. One was pointed at the road, and the other at the driver’s face. These videos were essential for annotations (see Section 4.1), so they were used to estimate the cognitive level of range anxiety.
- GPS antenna: Provided information for localization and vehicle heading. The HMI needed these data.
- LED stripes: they were also embedded in the vehicle’s A-pillars, piloted by the HMI. These LEDs changed color and animation according to the current HMI screen (i.e., normal driving mode, eco-mode, autonomous driving, and low battery).
5. Results
5.1. First Interview
5.2. Scenario Evaluation—Verbatim Exploitation
5.2.1. Initial Phases
5.2.2. Scenario A
5.2.3. Scenario B
5.2.4. Scenario C
5.3. Final Interview on User Experience
5.3.1. State of Mind on Emergency Stop
5.3.2. Revealing the Purpose
5.3.3. User Feeling on HMI
6. Conclusions
6.1. Discussion
6.2. Future Work
Author Contributions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A. Verbatim and Note List
Appendix A.1. Discovery—Initial Phase
HMI Actions or Events | Verbatim |
---|---|
WoZ autonomous driving | “It reacts nearly like a real human. It detects even little potholes and pays attention to them!” |
Positive Phase | Talking about these moments opened a better dialogue between participant and interviewer. The shared memories were often linked to road trips with friends or family. It also attenuated the anxiety of the participants, which might have been caused by being in an experiment. |
Neutral Driving | The experience of driving an EV was generally judged as “impressive”, “surprising”, and “pleasant”. The main factors that contributed to those judgments were the noisiness of the vehicle and its power (the torque motor). |
Appendix A.2. Scenario B
HMI Actions or Events | Verbatim |
---|---|
Initial range decrease | The first traces of anxiety were perceived from the participants who paid attention to the variation of the vehicle’s autonomy. They tried to calculate future consumption by comparing the EV’s battery consumption to a smartphone’s battery consumption. “We consumed a lot from the beginning of the experiment! We were at 59% at the beginning, and now we are at 42%. It discharges fast, I’ll try to adapt my driving style.” |
Hill pop-up | “This applies to the power consumption of a gasoline vehicle also, right? I already knew that.” |
Traffic light pop-up |
|
HMI color changes to orange | “I was supposed to arrive at the destination with 11% battery left. I am not sure anymore that I can.” |
Charging-station-proposal appearance on HMI |
|
Looking for a charging station | “The user interface is not bad at all for checking if the driving path is adapted in terms of distance and needed battery level.” |
Charging Station 1 | This first attempt to charge the EV was fruitless, as the infrastructure was out of order. Still, being stopped at that charging station gave the drivers the occasion and needed time to better explore the HMI. They then chose the second closest charging infrastructure. They felt the interface was there to help then, but they felt anxiety was increasingly perceivable. “What a pity that these charging stations are out of service. Do you know what the government plans are?” |
Charging Station 2 | After this second fruitless charging attempt, nervousness was palpable (weird jokes, vocal-tone changes, and anxious behaviors were observed and noted). Users were vigilant when driving after this point to adapt their driving behavior to be as economic as possible, following previous information that they had received from the HMI. “We will definitely be out of power! It has never happened to me before. We will have this experience together. I will adapt my driving style.” |
Appendix A.3. Scenario C
HMI Actions or Event | Verbatim |
---|---|
Route resumption (back to institute) | The HMI showed that it was always possible to reach the destination, and most of the drivers seemed to trust it despite their concerns and were willing to continue. Those who has concerns were told to trust the system and keep going. “You should add additional pedals to the back of the vehicle, then you could turn them to charge the battery like dynamos!” |
Autonomous Mode Safety Procedure | “Really? The vehicle will be fully autonomous in open roads? I want to accelerate more to discover how this technology would work in open roads!” |
Emergency Stop | This procedure was requested by the experts as soon as possible when autonomy was critical if people did not stop by themselves (originally, this procedure was planned with WoZ autonomous driving). However, the concern of being immobilized on the road was relativized, as drivers were not alone in the car. Similarly, being in city roads helped them to control their anxiety level, as assistance could easily be found in case of need. “If it’s like phones, it suddenly unloads, and we stopped in the middle of the road, we’re in trouble!” |
Calling Assistance | “Oh this is awesome! We can call the local assistance service directly from the HMI. We can even see the countdown to their arrival! I knew that this existed but I had never seen it before!” |
Call a taxi | “It is nice to have this option. Now we are on city roads so it is not that essential. But in rural areas that can help a lot.” |
Appendix B. Missing Data Issue
References
- Nilsson, M. Electric Vehicles the Phenomenon of Range Anxiety; Technical Report; Lindholmen Science Park: Lindholmen, Sweden, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Rauh, N.; Franke, T.; Krems, J.F. Understanding the Impact of Electric Vehicle Driving Experience on Range Anxiety. Hum. Factors 2015, 57, 177–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pearre, N.; Kempton, W.; Guensler, R.; Elango, V. Electric vehicles: How much range is required for a day’s driving? Transp. Res. Part C Emerg. Technol. 2011, 19, 1171–1184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Franke, T.; Krems, J.F. What drives range preferences in electric vehicle users? Transp. Policy 2013, 30, 56–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Knutsen, D.; Willén, O. A Study of Electric Vehicle Charging Patterns and Range Anxiety; Uppsala Universitet: Uppsala, Sweden, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Mayou, R.; Simken, S.; Threlfall, J. The effects of road traffic accidents on driving behavior. Injury 1991, 22, 365–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taylor, S.; Koch, W.J. Anxiety disorders due to motor vehicle accidents: Nature and treatment. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 1995, 15, 7211738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clapp, J.D.; Olsen, S.A.; Danoff-Burg, S.; Hagewood, J.H.; Hickling, E.J.; Hwang, V.S.; Beck, J.G. Factors contributing to anxious driving behavior: The role of stress history and accident severity. J. Anxiety Disord. 2011, 25, 592–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Moraveji, N.; Olson, B.; Nguyen, T.; Saadat, M.; Khalighi, Y.; Pea, R.; Heer, J. Peripheral Paced Respiration: Influencing User Physiology during Information Work. In Proceedings of the 24th ACM UIST Symposium, Santa Barbara, CA, USA, 16–19 October 2011; pp. 423–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sano, A.; Picard, R.W. Stress Recognition using Wearable Sensors and Mobile Phones. In Proceedings of the 2013 Humaine Association Conference on Affective Computing and Intelligent Interaction, Geneva, Switzerland, 2–5 September 2013; pp. 671–676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barajas, L.G. Behavior-Based Low Fuel Warning System. U.S. Patent US 7,999,664 B2, 16 August 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Tuzi, G.; Varughese, S. Low Fuel Warning. U.S. Patent US 9,163,953 B2, 20 October 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Cui, S.; Zhao, H.; Wen, H.; Zhang, C. Locating Multiple Size and Multiple Type of Charging Station for Battery Electricity Vehicles. Sustainability 2018, 10, 3267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Salah, K.; Kama, N. Reducing Range Anxiety by Unifying Networks of Charging Stations. ICMIT 2016, 70, 04003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bulut, E.; Kisacikoglu, M.C. Mitigating Range Anxiety via Vehicle-to-Vehicle Social Charging System. In Proceedings of the IEEE 85th Vehicular Technology Conference (VTC Spring), Sydney, NSW, Australia, 4–7 June 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Sarker, M.R.; Pandzic, H. Electric Vehicle Battery Swapping Station: Business Case and Optimization Model. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Connected Vehicles and Expo (ICCVE), Las Vegas, NV, USA, 2–6 December 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vaka, R.; Keshi, R.K. Review on Contactless Power Transfer for Electric. Energies 2017, 10, 636. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Yuan, Q.; Hao, W.; Su, H.; Bing, G.; Gui, X.; Safikhani, A. Investigation on Range Anxiety and Safety Buffer of Battery Electric Vehicle Drivers. J. Adv. Transp. 2018, 2018, 8301209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sarrafan, K.; Sutanto, D. An intelligent driver alerting system for real-time range indicator embedded in electric vehicles. In Proceedings of the IEEE Industry Applications Society Annual Meeting, Portland, OR, USA, 2–6 October 2016; pp. 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jung, M.F.; Sirkin, D.; Gür, T.M.; Steinert, M. Displayed Uncertainty Improves Driving Experience and Behavior: The Case of Range Anxiety in an Electric Car. In Proceedings of the 33rd Annual ACM Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Seoul, Korea, 18–23 April 2015; pp. 2201–2210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Khan, T.; Williams, M.; Wellings, T.; Robertson, D.; Binersley, J. Designing the Human Machine Interface to Address Range Anxiety. World Electr. Veh. J. 2012, 5, 72–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kuijsters, A.; Redi, J.; de Ruyter, B.; Heynderickx, I. Inducing Sadness and Anxiousness through Visual Media: Measurement Techniques and Persistence. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bosse, T.; Gerritsen, C.; de Man, J.; Stam, M. Inducing Anxiety through Video Material. Int. Conf. Hum.-Comput. Interact. 2014, 434, 301–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Louw, S. Active Listening in Qualitative Research Interviews. In Proceedings of the International Conference: Doing Research in Applied Linguistics, Bangkok, Thailand, 21–22 April 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Elias Hull Porter, J. The Development and Evaluation of a Measure of Counseling Interview Procedures’: Part I2 the Development. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 1943, 3, 105–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alhadreti, O.; Mayhew, P. “Thinking About Thinking Aloud”: An Investigation of Think-aloud Methods in Usability Testing. In Proceedings of the 30th International BCS Human Computer Interaction Conference: Companion Volume; BCS Learning & Development Ltd.: Swindon, UK, 2016; pp. 41:1–41:3. [Google Scholar]
- Sonderegger, A. Influencing Factors in Usability Tests: The Testing Situation, the Product Prototype, and the Test User. Ph.D. Thesis, Université de Fribourg, Fribourg, Sweden, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Gosling, S.D.; Rentfrow, P.J.; Swann, W.B., Jr. A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. J. Res. Person. 2003, 37, 504–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Likert, R. A technique for the measurement of attitudes. Arch. Psychol. 1932, 22, 140. [Google Scholar]
- Sullivan, G.M.; Artino, A.R. Analyzing and Interpreting Data From Likert-Type Scales. J. Grad. Med. Educ. 2013, 5, 541–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Martin, B.; Hanington, B.M. Universal Methods of Design; Rockport Publishers: Beverly, MA, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Wang, P.; Sibi, S.; Mok, B.K.; Ju, W. Marionette: Enabling On-Road Wizard-of-Oz Autonomous Driving Studies. In Proceedings of the 12th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI), Vienna, Austria, 6–9 March 2017; pp. 234–243. [Google Scholar]
- Hassenzahl, M.; Burmester, M.; Koller, F. AttrakDiff: Ein Fragebogen zur Messung wahrgenommener hedonischer und pragmatischer Qualität 1 Grundannahmen und Vorarbeiten. In Mensch & Computer; Springer: Stuttgart/Leipzig, Germany, 2003; pp. 187–196. [Google Scholar]
- Hassenzahl, M. The Interplay of Beauty, Goodness, and Usability in Interactive Products. Hum.-Comput. Interact. 2004, 19, 319–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Efentakis, A.; Grivas, N.; Lamprianidis, G.; Magenschab, G.; Pfoser, D. Isochrones, Traffic and DEMOgraphics. In Proceedings of the 21st ACM SIGSPATIAL International Conference on Advances in Geographic Information Systems, Orlando, FL, USA, 5–8 November 2013; pp. 548–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
HMI Actions or Events | Synthesis |
---|---|
Dynamic Car Discovery (WoZ autonomous driving) | Even though the cover to hide the joystick seemed an unusual feature, while reviewing the participants at the end of the experimentation, they declared being unaware that the researcher was steering the car (except for two of them). Experimenting with this system was a great success. On the basis of the noted verbatims, the drivers truly believed in the autonomous capability of the vehicle, and it seems that trust in the system was established only after a couple of minutes of autonomous driving. |
HMI Actions or Events | Synthesis |
---|---|
“Hill” and “Traffic light” pop-ups | The “Hill” coping strategy was perceived as an eco-driving mode. Some of the drivers judged this information to be useless, but still adapted their driving style as proposed to save energy. The “Traffic Light” coping strategy was understood (i.e., when to start the regenerative break when approaching to traffic light) and appreciated by all. However, some participants assigned it more complex meanings, such as the fact that the HMI was connected to the traffic light, and according to the remaining time to change the state, it anticipated the need to brake. |
HMI color changes to orange | HMI changed the color of the upper bar to orange when there was less than 40% battery left. This caught the attention of the drivers on the vehicle’s autonomy. |
Charging station proposal appearance on HMI | In this stage of the experiment, the remaining battery level was enough to reach point B. Therefore, the HMI did not offer a proposition to go to a charging station. It only activated a button that showed the closest charging stations. The drivers asked if they could charge the battery. Some of them detected by themselves how to find the closest charging stations through the HMI. They paid more attention to these details, especially when the vehicle was stopped at traffic lights for example (as this was the first time that they were using that interface). |
Looking for a charging station | This step only applied to drivers who did not feel the need to charge the battery. The experts started a short discussion between them about the remaining level of the battery as passenger concern to induce range anxiety in the driver. The driver questioned if they were correctly remembering the battery percentage at the beginning of the experiment. Majority of the participants remembered correctly the initial battery percentage (60%). Some of them told it was a little less than 60%. None of them claimed that the battery was charged more than 60%. |
HMI Actions or Event | Synthesis |
---|---|
Autonomous Mode Safety Procedure | This message often created confusion. The fact that the vehicle would inevitably take control was judged “surprising”. At the same time, this message reassured the drivers, as being blocked in the middle of a highway was not an acceptable option. Some drivers who believed the possibility that the vehicle would drive fully autonomously (like in the WoZ experiment from the positive phase) desired to push the battery to its extreme limits. |
Calling Assistance and Taxi | The assistance functionality was particularly appreciated by the drivers. It was also perceived as a “premium” functionality, in phase with the “technological progress of this century”. During the experiment, we prioritized making a fake phone call with the technician, who was supposed to rescue us with another vehicle, instead of using the “call a taxi” functionality. The presence of this choice, however, was noticed and appreciated as well. |
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Musabini, A.; Nguyen, K.; Rouyer, R.; Lilis, Y. Influence of Adaptive Human–Machine Interface on Electric-Vehicle Range-Anxiety Mitigation. Multimodal Technol. Interact. 2020, 4, 4. https://doi.org/10.3390/mti4010004
Musabini A, Nguyen K, Rouyer R, Lilis Y. Influence of Adaptive Human–Machine Interface on Electric-Vehicle Range-Anxiety Mitigation. Multimodal Technologies and Interaction. 2020; 4(1):4. https://doi.org/10.3390/mti4010004
Chicago/Turabian StyleMusabini, Antonyo, Kevin Nguyen, Romain Rouyer, and Yannis Lilis. 2020. "Influence of Adaptive Human–Machine Interface on Electric-Vehicle Range-Anxiety Mitigation" Multimodal Technologies and Interaction 4, no. 1: 4. https://doi.org/10.3390/mti4010004
APA StyleMusabini, A., Nguyen, K., Rouyer, R., & Lilis, Y. (2020). Influence of Adaptive Human–Machine Interface on Electric-Vehicle Range-Anxiety Mitigation. Multimodal Technologies and Interaction, 4(1), 4. https://doi.org/10.3390/mti4010004