Abstract
Three target-discrimination experiments were conducted to explore the spatial distribution t)f covert visual attention following an exogenous cue. On each trial, a brief peripheral onset was followed by a target stimulus in an otherwise empty visual field at one of eight (Experiment 1) or one of four (Experiments 2 and 3) possible locations centered at the fixation point. The spatial relation between the cue and the target was manipulated. The main results were that (1) performance was better at the cued location than at another nearby location in the same visual quadrant; (2) performance was not affected by the major horizontal and vertical visual meridians; and (3) performance was affected by the spatial distance between the cued and target locations. Together, the results suggest that the spatial distribution of exogenously oriented attention cart be most easily integrated with a simple gradient model.
Article PDF
Similar content being viewed by others
Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.
References
Abrams, R. A., Meyer, D. E., &Kornblum, S. (1989). Speed and accuracy of saccadic eye movements: Characteristics of impulse variability in the oculomotor system.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,15, 529–543.
Bouma, H. (1971). Visual recognition of isolated lower-case letters.Vision Research,11, 459–474.
Briand, K., &Klein, R. M. (1987). Is Posner’s “beam” the same as Treisman’s “glue”?: On the relation between visual orienting and feature integration theory.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,13, 228–241.
Downing, C. J. (1988). Expectancy and visuo-spatial attention: Effects on perceptual quality.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,14, 188–202.
Downing, C. J., &Pinker, S. (1985). The spatial structure of visual attention. In M. I. Posner & O. S. M. Marin (Eds.),Attention and performance XI (pp. 171–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Egly, R., &Homa, D. (1991). Reallocation of visual attention.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,17, 142–159.
Eriksen, C. W., &St. James, J. D. (1986). Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model.Perception & Psychophysics,40, 225–240.
Eriksen, C. W., &Webb, J. M. (1989). Shifting of attentional focus within and about a visual display.Perception & Psychophysics,45, 175–183.
Eriksen, C. W., &Yeh, Y. (1985). Allocation of attention in the visual field.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception Sc Performance,11, 583–597.
Henderson, J. M. (1988).Visual attention and the acquisition of extrafoveal information during eye fixations. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.
Henderson, J. M. (1991). Stimulus discrimination following covert attentional orienting to an exogenous cue.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,17, 91–106.
Henderson, J. M. (1992). Visual attention and eye movement control during reading and scene perception. In K. Rayner (Ed.),Eye movements and visual cognition: Scene perception and reading (pp. 260–283). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Henderson, J. M. (in press). The interface between perception and action: Evidence from eye movement control. In S. Davis & K. Aikens (Eds.),Vancouver studies in cognitive science: Vol. 4. Problems in perception. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Henderson, J. M., &Ferreira, F. (1990). The effect of foveal processing difficulty on the perceptual span in reading: Implications for attention and eye-movement control.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,16, 417–429.
Henderson, J. M., Pollatsek, A., &Rayner, K. (1987). Effects of foveal priming and parafoveal preview on object identification.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,13, 449–463.
Henderson, J. M., Pollatsek, A., &Rayner, K. (1989). Covert visual attention and extrafoveal information use during object identification.Perception & Psychophysics,45, 196–208,
Hughes, H. C., &Zimba, L. D. (1985). Spatial maps of directed visual attention.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,11, 409–430
Hughes, H. C., &Zimba, L. D. (1987). Natural boundaries for the spread of directed visual attention.Neuropsychologia,2, 5–18.
James, W. (1890).The principles of psychology (Vol. 1). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Jonides, J. (1981). Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind’s eye’s movement In J. Long & A. Baddeley (Eds.),Attention and performance IX pp. 187–203). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kjnsbourne, M. (1987). Mechanisms of unilateral neglect. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.),Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect. New York: North-Holland.
Klein, R., Kingstone, A., &Pontefract, A. (1992). Orienting of visual attention. In K. Rayner (Ed.),Eye movements and visual cognition: Scene perception and reading. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Klein, R., &Mccormick, P. (1989). Covert visual orienting: Hemifield activation can be mimicked by zoom lens and midlocation placement strategies.Acta Psychologica,70, 235–250.
Laberge, D., &Brown, V. (1989). Theory of attentional operations in shape identification.Psychological Review,96, 101–124.
Madden, D. J. (1992). Selective attention and visual search: Revision of an allocation model and application to age differences.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,18, 821–836.
Maylor, E. A. (1985). Facilitatory and inhibitory components of orienting in visual space. In M. I. Posner & O. S. M. Marin (Eds.),Attention and performance XI (pp. 189–204). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Mccormick, P. A., &Klein, R. (1990). The spatial distribution of attention during covert visual orienting.Acta Psychologica,75, 225–242.
Morrison, R. E. (1984). Manipulation of stimulus onset delay in reading: Evidence for parallel programming of saccades.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,10, 667–682.
Murphy, T. D., &Eriksen, C. W. (1987). Temporal changes in the distribution of attention in the visual field in response to precues.Perception & Psychophysics,42, 576–586.
Müller, H. J., &Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1989). Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: Time course of activation and resistance to interruption.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,15, 315–330.
Nakayama, K., &Mackeben, M. (1989). Sustained and transient components of focal visual attention.Vision Research,29, 1631–1647.
Pollatsek, A., &Rayner, K. (1990). Eye movements and lexical access in reading. In D. A. Balota, G. B. Flores d’Arcais, & K. Rayner (Eds.),Comprehension processes in reading (pp. 143–163). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,32, 3–25.
Posner, M. I., &Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. Bowhuis (Eds.),Attention and performance X (pp. 531–556). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R. R., &Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,109, 160–174.
Rafal, R. D., Calabresi, P. A., Brennan, C. W., &Sciolto, T. K. (1989). Saccadic preparation inhibits reorienting to recently attended locations.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,15, 673–685.
Rayner, K., Slowiaczek, M. L., Clifton, C., &Bertera, J. H. (1983). Latency of sequential eye movements: Implications for reading.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,9, 912–922.
Remington, R., &Pierce, L. (1984). Moving attention: Evidence for time-invariant shifts of visual selective attention.Perception & Psychophysics,35, 393–399.
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Kjnsbourne, M., &Moscovitch, M. (1990). Hemispheric control of spatial attention.Brain & Cognition,12, 240–266.
Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., Dascola, I., &Umiltà, C. (1987). Reorienting of attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention.Neuropsychologia,25, 31–40.
Shulman, G. L., Remington, R. W., &Mclean, J. P. (1979). Moving attention through visual space.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,5, 522–526.
Shulman, G. L., Wilson, J., &Sheehy, J. B. (1985). Spatial determinants of the distribution of attention.Perception & Psychophysics,37, 59–65.
Tassinari, G., Aguoti, S., Chelazzi, L., Marzi, C. A., &Berlucchi, G. (1987). Distribution in the visual field of the costs of voluntary allocated attention and of the inhibitory after-effects of covert orienting.Neuropsychologia,25, 55–71.
Treisman, A., &Gelade, G. (1980). A feature integration theory of attention.Cognitive Psychology,12, 97–136.
Tsal, Y. (1983). Movements of attention across the visual field.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,9, 523–530.
Tsal, Y. (1989). Do illusory conjunctions support the feature integration theory? A critical review of theory and findings.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,15, 394–400.
Yantis, S. (1988). On analog movements of visual attention.Perception & Psychophysics,43, 203–206.
Yantis, S., &Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Evidence from visual search.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,10, 601–621.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Additional information
The research reported in this article was supported by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Henderson, J.M., Macquistan, A.D. The spatial distribution of attention following an exogenous cue. Perception & Psychophysics 53, 221–230 (1993). https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211732
Received:
Accepted:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211732