Abstract
Animals adjust their behaviour in response to complex environmental conditions. This form of plasticity requires the formation of association between information and an appropriate behavioural response. Such a connection is the result of a complex interaction between evolutionary pre-programmed cue-response behaviour (innate behavioural response) and cumulated lifetime experience (learning). The evolution of learning and innate behavioural responses is likely to depend on their respective fitness costs and benefits. However, as natural selection will indirectly affect each form through global behavioural plasticity, it is critical to understand how each form interacts with the other. The inclusion of innate behavioural plasticity and learning in behaviour is likely to result in more than the mere sum of each plastic form. In this review we investigate the costs and benefits of learning and innate behavioural responses and the effect of one on the other in their evolution. We highlight the need for more explicit study of the interaction between innate behavioural response and learning in natural systems for a better understanding of behavioural plasticity.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Ackley D, Littman M (1990) Interactions between learning and evolution. In: Langton CG, Taylor C, Farmer JD, Rasmussen S (eds) 2nd workshop on artificial life. Santa Fe, Nm, pp 487–509
Ancel LW (2000) Undermining the Baldwin expediting effect: does phenotypic plasticity accelerate evolution? Theor Popul Biol 58:307–319
Anderson RW (1995) Learning and evolution: a quantitative genetics approach. J Theor Biol 175:89–101
Attwell D, Laughlin SB (2001) An energy budget for signaling in the grey matter of the brain. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 21:1133–1145
Baldwin JM (1896) A new factor in evolution. Am Nat 30:441–451
Barrickman NL, Bastian ML, Isler K, van Schaik CP (2008) Life history costs and benefits of encephalization: a comparative test using data from long-term studies of primates in the wild. J Hum Evol 54:568–590
Bateson P, Mameli M (2007) The innate and the acquired: useful clusters or a residual distinction from folk biology? Dev Psychobiol 49:818–831
Belew RK (1989) When both individuals and populations search: Adding simple learning to the genetic algorithm. In: Schaffer JD, Fairfax VA (eds) 3rd International Conf on Genetic Algorithms, pp 34–41
Berrigan D, Scheiner SM (2004) Modeling the evolution of phenotypic plasticity. In: DeWitt TJ, Scheiner SM (eds) Phenotypic plasticity: functional and conceptual approaches. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 82–97
Biegler R, McGregor A, Krebs JR, Healy SD (2001) A larger hippocampus is associated with longer-lasting spatial memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:6941–6944
Bolhuis JJ (1991) Mechanisms of avian imprinting: a review. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 66:303–345
Bolles RC (1970) Species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning. Psychol Rev 77:32–48
Bourret RB, Stock AM (2002) Molecular information processing: lessons from bacterial chemotaxis. J Biol Chem 277:9625–9628
Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press, Chicago
Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1988) The evolution of reciprocity in sizable groups. J Theor Biol 132:337–356
Brelands K, Brelands M (1961) The misbehavior of organisms. Am Psychol 16:681–684
Brown C, Laland KN (2003) Social learning in fishes: a review. Fish Fish 4:280–288
Burns JG, Rodd FH (2008) Hastiness, brain size and predation regime affect the performance of wild guppies in a spatial memory task. Anim Behav 76:911–922
Changizi MA (2003) Relationship between number of muscles, behavioral repertoire size, and encephalization in mammals. J Theor Biol 220:157–168
Crusio WE, Schwegler H, Brust I (1993) Covariations between hippocampal mossy fibres and working and reference memory in spatial and non-spatial radial maze tasks in mice. Eur J Neurosci 5:1413–1420
Dall SRX, Cuthill IC (1997) The information costs of generalism. Oikos 80:197–202
Davies NB, Madden JR, Butchart SHM, Rutila J (2006) A host-race of the cuckoo Cuculus canorus with nestlings attuned to the parental alarm calls of the host species. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 273:693–699
de Meester L (1996) Evolutionary potential and local genetic differentiation in a phenotypically plastic trait of a cyclical parthenogen, daphnia magna. Evolution 50:1293–1298
DeWitt TJ, Sih A, Wilson DS (1998) Costs and limits of phenotypic plasticity. Trends Ecol Evol 13:77–81
Domjam M, Wilson NE (1972) Specificity of cue to consequence in aversion learning in the rat. Psychon Sci 26:143–145
Dukas R (1998a) Cognitive ecology. University of Chicago Press, Chicago
Dukas R (1998b) Evolutionary ecology of learning. In: Dukas R (ed) Cognitive ecology: the evolutionary ecology of information processing and decision making. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 129–174
Dukas R (1999) Costs of memory: ideas and predictions. J Theor Biol 197:41–50
Dukas R (2008) Learning decreases heterospecific courtship and mating in fruit flies. Biol Lett 4:645–647
Fuller JL (1979) Fuller BWS lines: history and results. In: Hahn ME, Jensen C, Dudek BC (eds) Development and evolution of brain size: behavioral implications. Academic Press, New York, pp 187–204
Galef BG, Whiskin EE (2001) Interaction of social and individual learning in food preferences of Norway rats. Anim Behav 62:41–46
Garcia J, Koelling RA (1966) Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psychon Sci 4:123–124
Girvan JR, Braithwaite VA (1998) Population differences in spatial learning in three-spined sticklebacks. Proc R Soc Biol Sci Ser B 265:913–918
Grant BR, Grant PR (1996) Cultural inheritance of song and its role in the evolution of Darwin’s finches. Evolution 50:2471–2487
Gronenberg W, Liebig J (1999) Smaller brains and optic lobes in reproductive workers of the ant Harpegnathos. Naturwissenschaften 86:343–345
Gumbert A (2000) Color choices by bumble bees (Bombus terrestris): innate preferences and generalization after learning. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 48:36–43
Haier R, Karama S, Leyba L, Jung R (2009) MRI assessment of cortical thickness and functional activity changes in adolescent girls following three months of practice on a visual-spatial task. BMC Res Notes 2:174
Healy SD, Rowe C (2007) A critique of comparative studies of brain size. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 274:453–464
Heyes CM (1994) Social learning in animals: categories and mechanisms. Biol Rev 69:207–231
Hinton GE, Nowlan SJ (1987) How learning can guide evolution. Complex Systems 1:495–502
Hourcade B, Perisse E, Devaud J-M, Sandoz J-C (2009) Long-term memory shapes the primary olfactory center of an insect brain. Learn Mem 16:607–615
Isabel G, Pascual A, Preat T (2004) Exclusive consolidated memory phases in Drosophila. Science 304:1024–1027
Isler K, van Schaik CP (2006) Metabolic costs of brain size evolution. Biol Lett 2:557–560
Iwaniuk AN, Nelson JE (2003) Developmental differences are correlated with relative brain size in birds: a comparative analysis. Can J Zool 81:1913–1928
Jachner A (2001) Anti-predator behaviour of naive compared with experienced juvenile roach. J Fish Biol 59:1313–1322
Jacobs LF, Gaulin SJC, Sherry DF, Hoffman GE (1990) Evolution of spatial cognition—sex-specific patterns of spatial-behaviour predict hippocampal size. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 87:6349–6352
Jensen C (1979) Learning performance in mice genetically selected for brain weight: Problems of generality. In: Hahn ME, Jensen C, Dudek BC (eds) Development and evolution of brain size: behavioral implications. Academic Press, New York, pp 205–220
Johnston TD (1982) Selective costs and benefits in the evolution of learning. Adv Study Behav 12:65–106
Julian GE, Gronenberg W (2002) Reduction of brain volume correlates with behavioral changes in queen ants. Brain Behav Evol 60:152–164
Kats LB, Dill LM (1998) The scent of death: chemosensory assessment of predation risk by prey animals. Ecoscience 5:361–394
Lachlan RF, Servedio MR (2004) Song learning accelerates allopatric speciation. Evolution 58:2049–2063
Laughlin SB (2001) Energy as a constraint on the coding and processing of sensory information. Curr Opin Neurobiol 11:475–480
Laughlin SB, van Steveninck RRD, Anderson JC (1998) The metabolic cost of neural information. Nat Neurosci 1:36–41
Laverty TM, Plowright RC (1988) Flower handling by bumblebees: a comparison of specialists and generalists. Anim Behav 36:733–740
Lefebvre L, Giraldeau LA (1996) Is social learning an adaptive specialization? In: Galef BG, Heyes CM (eds) Social learning in animals: the roots of culture. Academic Press, New York, pp 107–128
Magurran AE (1986) The development of shoaling behavior in the European minnow, Phoxinus phoxinus. J Fish Biol 29:159–169
Marino L (2005) Big brains do matter in new environments. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:5306–5307
Mayley G (1997) Guiding or hiding: explorations into the effects of learning on the rate of evolution. In: Husbands P, Harvey I (eds) 4th European conference on artificial life (ECAL97). Brighton, England, pp 135–144
Mery F, Kawecki TJ (2002) Experimental evolution of learning ability in fruit flies. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99:14274–14279
Mery F, Kawecki TJ (2004a) The effect of learning on experimental evolution of resource preference in Drosophila melanogaster. Evolution 58:757–767
Mery F, Kawecki TJ (2004b) An operating cost of learning in Drosophila melanogaster. Anim Behav 68:589–598
Mery F, Kawecki TJ (2005) A cost of long-term memory in Drosophila. Science 308:1148
Moran NA (1992) The evolutionary maintenance of alternative phenotypes. Am Nat 139:971–989
Morgan CL (1896) On modification and variation. Science 4:733–740
Moscovitch A, Lolordo VM (1968) Role of safety in pavlovian backward fear conditioning procedure. J Comp Physiol Psychol 66: 673
Nelson DA (2000) A preference for own-subspecies’ song guides vocal learning in a song bird. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 97:13348–13353
Niven JE, Anderson JC, Laughlin SB (2007) Fly photoreceptors demonstrate energy-information trade-offs in neural coding. PLoS Biol 5:828–840
Odling-Smee L, Braithwaite VA (2003) The influence of habitat stability on landmark use during spatial learning in the three-spined stickleback. Anim Behav 65:701–707
Papaj DR (1994) Optimizing learning and its effects on evolutionary change. In: Real L (ed) Behavioral mechanisms in evolutionary ecology. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 133–153
Papaj DR, Prokopy RJ (1989) Ecological and evolutionary aspects of learning in phytophagous insects. Annu Rev Entomol 34:315–350
Pigliucci M (2005) Evolution of phenotypic plasticity: where are we going now? Trends Ecol Evol 20:481–486
Pitnick S, Jones KE, Wilkinson GS (2006) Mating system and brain size in bats. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 273:719–724
Podos J, Huber SK, Taft B (2004) Bird song: the interface of evolution and mechanism. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst 35:55–87
Pravosudov VV, Clayton NS (2002) A test of the adaptive specialization hypothesis: population differences in caching, memory, and the hippocampus in black—capped chickadees (Poecile atricapilla). Behav Neurosci 116:515–522
Ratcliffe JM, Fenton MB, Galef BG (2003) An exception to the rule: common vampire bats do not learn taste aversions. Anim Behav 65:385–389
Relyea RA (2001) The relationship between predation risk and antipredator responses in larval anurans. Ecology 82:541–554
Reznick DN, Ghalambor CK (2005) Selection in nature: experimental manipulations of natural populations. Integr Comp Biol 45:456–462
Riffell JA, Alarcon R, Abrell L, Davidowitz G, Bronstein JL, Hildebrand JG (2008) Behavioral consequences of innate preferences and olfactory learning in hawkmoth-flower interactions. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105:3404–3409
Robinson BW, Dukas R (1999) The influence of phenotypic modifications on evolution: the Baldwin effect and modern perspectives. Oikos 85:582–589
Rosenzweig MR, Bennett EL (1996) Psychobiology of plasticity: effects of training and experience on brain and behavior. Behav Brain Res 78:57–65
Roudez RJ, Glover T, Weis JS (2008) Learning in an invasive and a native predatory crab. Biol Invasions 10:1191–1196
Sacher GA, Staffeldt EF (1974) Relation of gestation time to brain weight for placental mammals: implications for theory of vertebrate growth. Am Nat 108:593–615
Sasaki K, Fox SF, Duvall D (2009) Rapid evolution in the wild: changes in body size, life-history traits, and behavior in hunted populations of the Japanese mamushi snake. Conserv Biol 23:93–102
Sherry DF, Hoshooley JS (2009) The seasonal hippocampus of food-storing birds. Behav Processes 80:334–338
Shettleworth SJ (1998) Cognition, evolution, and behavior. Oxford University Press, New York
Smid HM, Bukovinszky T, Wang G, Steidle JLM, Bleeker MAK, van Loon JJA, Vet LEM (2007) Species-specific acquisition and consolidation of long-term memory in parasitic wasps. Proc R Soc Biol Sci Ser B 274:1539–1546
Snell-Rood EC, Papaj DR (2009) Patterns of phenotypic plasticity in common and rare environments: a study of host use and color learning in the cabbage white butterfly Pieris rapae. Am Nat 173:615–631
Snell-Rood EC, Papaj DR, Gronenberg W (2009) Brain size: a global or induced cost of learning? Brain Behav Evol 73:111–128
Sokoloff L (1960) The metabolism of the central nervous system in vivo. In: Field L, Magoun H, Hall VE (eds) Handbook of physiology. American Physiological Society, Washington, pp 1843–1864
Sol D (2009) Revisiting the cognitive buffer hypothesis for the evolution of large brains. Biol Lett 5:130–133
Sol D, Duncan RP, Blackburn TM, Cassey P, Lefebvre L (2005) Big brains, enhanced cognition, and response of birds to novel environments. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:5460–5465
Stearns SC (1992) The evolution of life histories. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Stephens D (1991) Change, regularity, and value in the evolution of animal learning. Behav Ecol 2:77–89
Sullivan KA (1988) Age-specific profitability and prey choice. Anim Behav 36:613–615
Tanimoto H, Heisenberg M, Gerber B (2004) Event timing turns punishment to reward. Nature 430:983
Turner AM, Fetterolf SA, Bernot RJ (1999) Predator identity and consumer behavior: differential effects of fish and crayfish on the habitat use of a freshwater snail. Oecologia 118:242–247
Van Buskirk J (2002) Phenotypic lability and the evolution of predator-induced plasticity in tadpoles. Evolution 56:361–370
van Praag H, Kempermann G, Gage FH (2000) Neural consequences of environmental enrichment. Nat Rev Neurosci 1:191–198
West-Eberhard MJ (2003) Developmental plasticity and evolution. Oxford University Press, New York
Acknowledgments
We thank N. Kohn and C. Reaume for useful comments on the manuscript. The work was supported by an ATIP Grant from the Life Sciences Division of the Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique and from the European Research Council under the European Community’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2013)/ERC Grant agreement no 209540.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Mery, F., Burns, J.G. Behavioural plasticity: an interaction between evolution and experience. Evol Ecol 24, 571–583 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10682-009-9336-y
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10682-009-9336-y