Abstract
The shared attention theory suggests that people devote greater cognitive resources to those features co-attended simultaneously with others, determining better performance in several types of tasks. When co-actors performed a go/no-go Navon task attending different features of target letters, the performance was impaired, reflecting a joint Navon effect (the representation of a co-actor’s attentional focus made it more difficult to select and apply one’s own focus of attention), probably due to asynchronous co-attention with a decrease in cognitive resources involved. Researches in chronobiology and chronopsychology demonstrated that not only selective attention (involved in a Navon task), but also cognitive resources have a daily fluctuations, mainly paralleling the circadian rhythm of body temperature (i.e. increasing values from the morning to evening with a subsequent decline in the night). The study was conducted to assess whether the presence of joint attention, as measured by the joint Navon effect, was influenced by the time-of-day. Sixteen pairs of participants sitting next to each other were required to respond to the identity letters in a go/no-go Navon task twice: in the morning (09:00–10:00) and early afternoon (13:00–14:00). The results showed a joint Navon effect in the morning session only, suggesting that joint attention was affected by the time-of-day effect on cognitive resources.
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.References
Adan A (1992) Validation of axillary temperature measurement as a biorhythm marker: study of the sex variable. Arch Neurobiol 55:103–111
Adan A (1993) Circadian variations in psychological measures: a new classification. Chronobiologia 20:145–161
Adan A, Archer SN, Hidalgo MP, DiMilia L, Natale V, Randler C (2012) Circadian typology: a comprehensive review. Chronobiol Int 29:1153–1175. https://doi.org/10.3109/07420528.2012.719971
Ahmed L, de Fockert JW (2012) Working memory load can both improve and impair selective attention: evidence from the Navon paradigm. Atten Percept Psychophys 74:1397–1405. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-012-0357-1
Amodio DM, Frith CD (2006) Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:268–277. https://doi.org/10.1038/nm1884
Anderson JAE, Campbell LL, Amer T, Grady CL, Hasher L (2014) Timing is everything: age differences in the cognitive control network are modulated by time of day. Psychol Aging 29:648–657. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037243
Atmaca S, Sebanz N, Prinz W, Knoblich G (2008) Action co-representation: the joint SNARC effect. Soc Neurosci 3:410–420. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470910801900908
Atmaca S, Sebanz N, Knoblich G (2011) The joint flanker effect: sharing tasks with real and imagined co-actors. Exp Brain Res 211:371–385. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2709-9
Baddeley A (2003) Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nat Rev Neurosci 4:829–839. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1201
Baehr EK, Revelle W, Eastman CI (2000) Individual differences in the phase and amplitude of the human circadian temperature rhythm: with an emphasis on morningness-eveningness. J Sleep Res 9:117–127
Bennett CL, Petros TV, Johnson M, Ferraro FR (2008) Individual differences in the influence of time of day on executive functions. Am J Psychol 121:349–361
Blatter K, Cajochen C (2007) Circadian rhythms in cognitive performance: methodological constraints, protocols, theoretical underpinnings. Physiol Behav 90:196–2008. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.09.009
Böckler A, Sebanz N (2012) A co-actor’s focus of attention affects stimulus processing and task performance: an ERP study. Soc Neurosci 7:565–577. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2012.682119
Böckler A, Knoblich G, Sebanz N (2012) Effects of a coactor’s focus of attention on task performance. J Exp Psychol Human 38:1404–1415. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027523
Bodenhausen GV (1990) Stereotypes as judgmental heuristics: evidence of circadian variations in discrimination. Psychol Sci 1:319–322. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00226.x
Borbély AA (1982) A two process model of sleep regulation. Hum Neurobiol 1:195–204
Borbély AA, Daan S, Wirz-Justice A, Deboer T (2016) The two-process model of sleep regulation: a reappraisal. J Sleep Res 25:131–143. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12371
Borella E, Ludwig C, Dirk J, de Ribaupierre A (2010) The influence of time of testing on interference, working memory, processing speed, and vocabulary: age differences in adulthood. Exp Aging Res 37:76–107. https://doi.org/10.1080/0361073X.2011.536744
Botvinick MM, Braver TS, Barch DM, Carter CS, Cohen JD (2001) Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol Rev 108:624–652. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624
Brennan SE, Chen X, Dickinson CA, Nieder MB, Zelinsky GJ (2007) Coordinating cognition: the costs and benefits of shared gaze during collaborative search. Cognition 106:1465–1477. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.05.012
Broadbent DE, Broadbent MHP, Jones JL (1989) Time of day as an instrument for the analysis of attention. Eur J Cognit Psychol 1:69–94. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541448908403072
Browman CP, Sullivan HT (1980) Diurnal periodicity of the human vertex potential. Percept Mot Skills 51:642–646. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1980.51.2.643
Butterfill SA, Sebanz N (2011) Joint action: what is shared? Rev Philos Psychol 2:137–146. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-011-0062-3
Cajochen C, Foy R, Dijk DJ (1999a) Frontal predominance of a relative increase in sleep delta and theta EEG activity after sleep loss in humans. Sleep Res Online 2:65–69
Cajochen C, Khalsa SB, Wyatt JK, Czeisler CA, Dijk DJ (1999b) EEG and ocular correlates of circadian melatonin phase and human performance decrements during sleep loss. Am J Physiol 277:R640–R649
Campbell SS (1995) Effects of timed bright-light exposure on shift-work adaptation in middle-aged subjects. Sleep 18:408–416
Carrier J, Monk TH (2000) Circadian rhythms of performance: new trends. Chronobiol Int 17:719–732
Carter CS, MacDonald AW 3rd, Ross LL, Stenger VA (2001) Anterior cingulate cortex activity and impaired self-monitoring of performance in patients with schizophrenia: an event-related fMRI study. Am J Psychiatry 158:1423–1428. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.aip.158.9.1423
Cavanaugh LA, Cutright KM, Frances Luce M, Bettmann JE (2011) Hope, pride, and processing during optimal and nonoptimal times of day. Emotion 11:38–46. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022016
Corbera X, Grau C, Vendrell P (1993) Diurnal oscillations in hemisphere performance. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 15:300–310. https://doi.org/10.1080/01688639308402564
Correa A, Lara T, Madrid JA (2013) Influence of circadian typology and time of day on temporal preparation. Timing Time Percept 1:217–238. https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-00002012
Correa A, Molina E, Sanabria D (2014) Effects of chronotype and time of day on the vigilance decrement during simulated driving. Accid Anal Prev 67:113–118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2014.02.020
Correa A, Ruiz-Herrera N, Ruz M, Tonetti L, Martoni M, Fabbri M, Natale V (2017) Economic decision-making in morning/evening-type people as a function of time of day. Cronobiol Int 39:139–147. https://doi.org/10.1080/07420528.2016.1246455
Dale G, Arnell KM (2010) Individual differences in dispositional focus of attention predict attentional blink magnitude. Atten Percept Psychophys 72:602–606. https://doi.org/10.3758/APP.72.3.602
Dale G, Arnell KM (2013) Investigating the stability of and relationships among global/local processing measures. Atten Percept Psychophys 75:394–406. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-012-0416-7
de Fockert JW, Rees G, Frith CD, Lavie N (2001) The role of working memory in visual selective attention. Science 291:1803–1806. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1056496
Dickinson DL, McElroy T (2012) Circadian effects on strategic reasoning. Exp Econ 15:444–459. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-011-9307-3
Dinges DF (1995) An overview of sleepiness and accidents. J Sleep Res 4:4–14. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.1995.tb00220.x
Dolk T, Hommel B, Colzato LS, Schütz-Bosbach S, Prinz W, Liepelt R (2014) The joint Simon effect: a review and theoretical integration. Front Psychol 5:974. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00974
Duffy JF, Dijk DJ (2002) Getting through to circadian oscillators: why use constant routines? J Biol Rhythm 17:4–13
Fabbri M, Martoni M, Esposito MJ, Brighetti G, Natale V (2006) Postural control after a night without sleep. Neuropsychologia 44:2520–2525. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.033
Fabbri M, Natale V, Adan A (2008) Effects of time of day on arithmetic fact retrieval in a number-matching task. Acta Psychol 127:485–490. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.act.psy.2007.08.011
Fabbri M, Mencarelli C, Adan A, Natale V (2013) Time-of-day and circadian typology on memory retrieval. Biol Rhythm Res 44:125–142. https://doi.org/10.1018/09291016.2012.656244
Fan J, Flombaum JI, McCandliss BD, Thomas KM, Posner MI (2003) Cognitive and brain consequences of conflict. NeuroImage 18:42–57. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1319
Fimm B, Brand T, Spijkers W (2016) Time-of-day variation of visuo-spatial attention. Br J Psychol 107:299–321. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12143
Fink GR, Halligan PW, Marshall JC, Frith C, Frackowiak R, Dolan RJ (1996) Where in the brain does visual attention select the forest and the tree? Nature 382:626–628. https://doi.org/10.1038/382626a0
Fink GR, Halligan PW, Marshall JC, Frith CD, Frackowiak RSJ, Dolan RJ (1997) Neural mechanisms involved in the processing of global and local aspects of hierarchical organized visual stimuli. Brain 120:1779–1791
Fink GR, Marshall JC, Halligan PW, Dolan RJ (1999) Hemispheric asymmetries in global/local processing are modulated by perceptual salience. Neuropsychologia 37:31–40
Folkard S (1979) Time of day and level of processing. Mem Cogn 7:247–252. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197596
Folkard S, Knauth P, Monk TH (1976) The effect of memory load on the circadian variation in performance efficiency under a rapidly rotating shift system. Ergonomics 19:479–488. https://doi.org/10.1080/00140137608931559
Folkard S, Wever RA, Wildgruber CM (1983) Multi-oscillatory control of circadian rhythms in human performance. Nature 305:223–226. https://doi.org/10.1038/305223a0
Fougnie D (2008) The relationship between attention and working memory. In: Johansen NB (ed) New research on short-term memory. Nova Science Publisher, New York, pp 1–45
Garavan H, Ross TJ, Murphy K, Roche RA, Stein EA (2002) Dissociable executive functions in the dynamic control of behaviour: inhibition, error detection, and correction. NeuroImage 17:1820–1829. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1326
García A, Ramírez C, Martínez B, Valdez P (2012) Circadian rhythms in two components of executive functions: cognitive inhibition and flexibility. Biol Rhythm Res 43:49–63. https://doi.org/10.1080/09291016.2011.638137
Geisser S, Greenhouse SW (1958) An extension of Box’s results on the use of the F distribution in multivariate analysis. Ann Math Stat 29:885–891
Greenhouse SW, Geisser S (1959) On methods in the analysis of profile data. Psychometrika 24:95–112. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02289823
Hahn C, Cowell JM, Wiprzycka UJ, Goldstein D, Ralph M, Hasher L, Zelazo PD (2012) Circadian rhythms in executive function during the transition to adolescence: the effect of synchrony between chronotype and time of day. Dev Sci 15:408–416. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01137.x
Han S, Weaver JA, Murray SO, Kang X, Yund EW, Woods DL (2002) Hemispheric asymmetry in global/local processing: effects of stimulus position and spatial frequency. NeuroImage 17:1290–1299. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1255
Harrison Y, Horne JA (2000) The impact of sleep deprivation on decision making: a review. J Exp Psychol Appl 6:236–249. https://doi.org/10.1037//1076-808X.6.3.236
Harrison Y, Jones K, Waterhouse J (2007) The influence of time awake and circadian rhythm upon performance on a frontal lobe task. Neuropsychologia 45:1966–1972. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.12.012
Hasher L, Lusting C, Zacks RT (2007) Inhibitory mechanisms and the control of attention. In: Conway AR, Jarrod C, Kane MJ, Miyake A, Towse JN (eds) Variation in working memory. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 227–249
He X, Lever A, Humphreys GW (2011) Interpersonal memory-based guidance of attention is reduced for ingroup members. Exp Brain Res 211:429–438. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2698-8
Hedgé J (2008) Time course of visual processing: coarse-to-fine processing and beyond. Prog Neurobiol 84:405–439. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2007.09.001
Hester R, Fassbender C, Garavan H (2004) Individual differences in error processing: a review and reanalysis of three event-related fMRI studies using the go/nogo task. Cereb Cortex 14:986–994. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhh059
Horowitz TS, Cade BE, Wolfe JM, Czeisler CA (2003) Searching night and day: a dissociation of effects of circadian phase and time awake on visual selective attention and vigilance. Psychol Sci 14:549–557. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1464.x
Hossain MT, Saini R (2014) Suckers in the morning, skeptics in the evening: time-of-day effects on consumers’ vigilance against manipulation. Mark Lett 25:109–121. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-013-9247-0
Kimchi R (1992) Primacy of wholistic processing and global/local paradigm: a critical review. Psychol Bull 112:24–38. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.24
Kimchi R, Palmer SE (1982) Form and texture in hierarchically constructed patterns. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 8:521–535. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.8.4.521
Kleitman N, Titelbaum S, Feiveson P (1938) The effect of body temperature on reaction time. Am J Physiol Leg Content 121:495–501
Knight M, Mather M (2013) Look out – it’s your off-peak time of day! Time of day matters more for alerting than for orienting or executive attention. Exp Aging Res 39:305–321. https://doi.org/10.1080/0361073X.2013.779197
Knoblich G, Sebanz N (2006) The social nature of perception and action. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 15:99–104. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00415.x
Knoblich G, Butterfill S, Sebanz N (2011) Psychological research on joint action: theory and data. In: Ross B (ed) The psychology of learning and motivation. Academic Press, Burlington, pp 59–101
Kourtis D, Knoblich G, Woźniak Sebanz N (2014) Attention allocation and task representation during joint action planning. J Cognit Neurosci 26:2275–2286. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00634
Kraemer S, Danker-Hopfe H, Dorn H, Schmidt A, Ehlert I, Herrmann WM (2000) Time-of-day variations of indicators of attention: performance, physiologic parameters, and self-assessment of sleepiness. Biol Psychiatry 48:1069–1080. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00908-2
Krämer UM, Solbakk A-K, Funderud I, Løvstad M, Endestad T, Knight RT (2013) The role of the lateral prefrontal cortex in inhibitory motor control. Cortex 49:837–849. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2012.05.003
Kruglanski AW, Pierro A (2008) Night and day, you are the one: on circadian mismatches and the transference effect in social perception. Psychol Sci 19:296–301. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02083.x
Lack LC, Lushington K (1996) The rhythms of human sleep propensity and core body temperature. J Sleep Res 5:1–11. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2869.1996.00005-x
Lamb MR, Robertson LC, Knight RT (1989) Attention and interference in the processing of global and local information: effects of unilateral temporal-parietal junction lesions. Neuropsychologia 27:471–483. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(89)90052-3
Lamb MR, Robertson LC, Knight RT (1990) Component mechanisms underlying the processing of hierarchically organized patterns: interferences from patients with unilateral cortical lesions. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cognit 16:471–483. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.16.3.471
Lara T, Madrid JA, Correa A (2014) The vigilance decrement in executive function is attenuated when individual chronotypes perform at their optimal time of day. PLoS ONE 9:e88820. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088820
Lavie N, Hirst A, de Fockert JW, Viding E (2004) Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. J Exp Psychol Gen 133:339–354. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.339
Lehnkering H, Strauss A, Wegner B, Siegmund R (2006) Actigraphic investigations on the activity-rest behavior of right- and left-handed students. Chronobiol Int 23:593–605. https://doi.org/10.1080/07420520600724094
Lux S, Marshall JC, Ritzl A, Weiss PH, Pietrzyk U, Shah NJ et al (2004) A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of local/global processing with stimulus presentation in the peripheral visual hemifields. Neuroscience 124:113–120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2003.10.044
Manly T, Lewis GH, Robertson IH, Watson PC, Datta AK (2002) Coffee in the cornflakes: time-of-day as a modulator of executive response control. Neuropsychologia 40:1–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00086-0
Marek T, Fafrowicz M, Golonka K, Mojsa-Kaja J, Oginska H, Tucholska K, Urbanik A, Beldzik E, Domagalik A (2010) Diurnal patterns of activity of the orienting and executive attention neuronal networks in subjects performing a Stroop-like task: a functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging study. Chronobiol Int 27:915–958. https://doi.org/10.3109/07420528.2010.489400
May CP, Hasher L (1998) Synchrony effects in inhibitory control thought and action. J Exp Psycho Hum Percep Perform 24:363–379. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.24.2.363
McGrath JE, Kelly JR (1986) Time and human interaction: toward a social psychology of time. Guilford, New York
Milham MP, Erickson KI, Banich MT, Kramer AF, Webb A, Wszalek T, Cohen NJ (2002) Attentional control in the aging brain: insights from an fMRI study of the stroop task. Brain Cognit 49:277–296. https://doi.org/10.1006/brcg.2001.1501
Miller J, Navon D (2002) Global precedence and response activation: evidence from LRPs. Q J Exp Psychol A 55:289–310. https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980143000280
Mitler MM, Carskadon MA, Czeisler CA, Dement WC, Dinges DF, Graeber RC (1988) Catastrophes, sleep, and public policy: consensus report. Sleep 11:100–109
Mongrain V, Carrier J, Dumont M (2006) Circadian and homeostatic sleep regulation in morningness-eveningness. J Sleep Res 15:162–166. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2006.00532.x
Monk TH (1989) A visual analogue scale technique to measure global vigor and affect. Psychiatry Res 27:89–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/0165-1781(89)90013-9
Monk TH (1997) Circadian rhythms in human performance and mood under constant conditions. J Sleep Res 6:9–18. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2869.1197.00023.x
Mundy P, Newell L (2007) Attention, joint attention and social cognition. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 16:269–274. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00518.x
Natale V, Cicogna PC (1996) Circadian regulation of subjective alertness in morning and evening types. Per Indiv Differ 20:491–497. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(95)00213-8
Natale V, Alzani A, Cicogna PC (2003) Cognitive efficiency and circadian typologies: a diurnal study. Per Indiv Differ 35:1089–1105. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00320-3
Natale V, Martoni M, Esposito MJ, Fabbri M, Tonetti L (2007) Circadian motor asymmetries before and after prolonged wakefulness in humans. Neurosci Lett 423:216–218. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2007.07.026
Navon D (1977) Forest before trees: the precedence of global features in visual processing. Cognit Psychol 9:353–383. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3
Navon D (1981) The forest revisited: more on global precedence. Psychol Res 43:1–32. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00309635
Navon D (1991) Testing a queue hypothesis for the processing of global and local information. J Exp Psychol Gen 120:173–189. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.120.2.173
Navon D (2003) What does a compound letter tell the psychologist’s mind? Acta Psychol 114:273–309. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.06.002
Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9:97–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4
Pomplun M, Silva EJ, Ronda JM, Cain W, Münch MY, Czeisler CA, Duffy JF (2012) The effects of circadian phase, time awake, and imposed sleep restriction on performing complex visual tasks: evidence from comparative visual search. J Vis 12:14. https://doi.org/10.1167/12.7.14
Ramirez C, Talamantes J, Garcia A, Morales M, Valdez P, Menna-Barreto L (2006) Circadian rhythms in phonological and visuospatial storage components of working memory. Biol Rhythm Res 37:433–441. https://doi.org/10.1080/09291010600870404
Reinberg A, Bicakova-Rocher A, Mechkouri M, Ashkenazi I (2002) Right- and left-brain hemisphere. Rhythm in reaction time to light signals is task-load-dependent: age, gender and handgrip strength rhythm comparison. Chronobiol Int 19:1087–1106
Richardson JTE (2011) Eta squared and partial eta squared as measures of effect size in educational research. Educ Res Rev 6:135–147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2010.12.001
Robertson LC, Lamb MR (1991) Neuropsychological contributions to theories of part/whole organization. Cognit Psychol 23:299–330. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(91)90012-D
Schmidt C, Collette F, Cajochen C, Peigneux P (2007) A time to think: circadian rhythms in human cognition. Cognit Neuropsychol 24:755–789. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290701754158
Schmidt C, Peigneux P, Leclercq Y, Sterpenich V, Vandewalle G, Phillips C et al (2012) Circadian preference modulates the neural substrate of conflict processing across the day. PLoS ONE 7:e29658. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0029658
Sebanz N, Knoblich G (2009) Prediction in joint action: what, when, and where. Top Cognit Sci 1:353–367. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01024.x
Sebanz N, Knoblich G, Prinz W (2003) Representing others’ actions: just like one’s own? Cognition 88:B11–B21. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00043-X
Sebanz N, Knoblich G, Prinz W (2005) How two share a task: corepresenting stimulus-response mappings. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 31:1234–1246. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.31.6.1234
Sebanz N, Bakkering H, Knoblich G (2006) Joint action: bodies and minds moving together. Trends Cognit Sci 10:70–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.12.009
Shteynberg G (2015) Shared attention. Perspect Psychol Sci 10:579–590. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615589104
Simon JR (1969) Reactions toward the source of stimulation. J Exp Psychol 81:174–176. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0027448
Tankova I, Adan A, Buela-Casal G (1994) Circadian typology and individual differences: a review. Personal Indiv Differ 16:671–684. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(94)90209-7
Tipper SP (2010) From observation to action simulation: the role of attention, eye-gaze, emotion, and body state. Q J Exp Psychol 63:2081–2105. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470211003624002
Tomasello M, Carpenter M (2007) Shared intentionality. Dev Sci 10:121–125. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00573.x
Valdez P, Ramírez C, García A, Talamantes J, Armijo P, Borrani J (2005) Circadian rhythms in components of attention. Biol Rhythm Res 36:57–65. https://doi.org/10.1080/09291010400028633
Valdez P, Ramírez C, García A, Talamantes J, Cortez J (2010) Circadian and homeostatic variation in sustained attention. Chronobiol Int 27:393–416. https://doi.org/10.3109/07420521003765861
Van Dongen H, Dinges DF (2005) Circadian rhythms in sleepiness, alertness, and performance. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC (eds) Principles and practice in sleep medicine, 4th edn. WB Saunders, Philadelphia, pp 435–443
Vesper C, Abramova E, Bütepage J, Ciardo F, Crossey B, Effenberg A et al (2017) Joint action: mental representations, shared information and general mechanisms for coordinating with others. Front Psychol 7:2039. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02039
Waterhouse J, Drust B, Weinert D, Edwards B, Gregson W, Atkinson G, Keo S, Aizawa S, Reilly T (2005) The circadian rhythm of core temperature: origin and some implications for exercise performance. Chronobiol Int 22:207–225
Weinert D, Waterhouse J (2007) The circadian rhythm of core temperature: effects of physical activity and aging. Physiol Behav 90:246–256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.09.003
Weissman DH, Giesbrecht B, Song AW, Mangun GR, Woldorff MG (2003) Conflict monitoring in the human anterior cingulate cortex during selective attention to global and local object features. NeuroImage 19:1361–1368. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00167-8
Wenke D, Atmaca S, Holländer A, Liepelt R, Baess P, Prinz W (2011) What is shared in joint action? Issues of co-representation, response conflict, and agent identification. Rev Philos Psychol 2:147–172. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-011-0057-0
West R, Murphy KJ, Armilio ML, Craik FIM, Stuss DT (2002) Effects of time of day on age differences in working memory. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci 57B:p3–p10
Wieth MB, Zacks RT (2011) Time of day effects on problem solving: when the non-optimal is optimal. Think Reason 17:387–401. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2011.625663
Wright KP, Hull JT, Czeisler CA (2002) Relationship between alertness, performance, and body temperature in humans. Am J Physiol-Regul Integr Comp Physilol 283:R1370–R1377. https://doi.org/10.1152/ajpregu.00205.2002
Wyatt JK, Ritz-De Cecco A, Czeisler CA, Dijk DJ (1999) Circadian temperature and melatonin rhythms, sleep, and neurobehavioral function in humans living on a 20-h day. Am J Physiol 277:R1152–R1163
Yovel G, Yovel I, Levy J (2001) Hemispheric asymmetries for global and local visual perception: effects of stimulus and task factors. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 27:1369–1385. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.27.6.1369
Zani A (1986) Time-of-day preference, pattern evoked potentials, and hemispheric asymmetries: a preliminary statement. Percept Mot Skills 63:413–414. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1986.63.2.413
Zimmerman P, Görtelmeyer R, Wieman H (1983) Diurnal periodicity of lateral asymmetries of the visual evoked potential in healthy volunteers. Neuropsychobiology 9:178–181. https://doi.org/10.1159/000117960
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Ethical approval
All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. In addition, the study received positive approval by the Bioethics Committee of the University of Bologna.
Informed consent
Written informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Additional information
Handling editor: Fabiano Botta (University of Granada). Reviewers: Diana Martella (Autonomous University of Chile), Pablo Valdez (Autonomous University of Nuevo Leon).
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Fabbri, M., Frisoni, M., Martoni, M. et al. Influence of time-of-day on joint Navon effect. Cogn Process 19, 27–40 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-017-0849-y
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-017-0849-y