Abstract
Sex differences in visuospatial abilities are long debated. Men generally outperform women, especially in wayfinding or learning a route or a sequence of places. These differences might depend on women’s disadvantage in underlying spatial competences, such as mental rotation, and on the strategies used, as well as on emotions and on self-belief about navigational skills, not related to actual skill-levels. In the present study, sex differences in visuospatial and navigational working memory in emotional contexts were investigated. Participants’ mood was manipulated by background music (positive, negative or neutral) while performing on the Corsi Block-tapping Task (CBT) and Walking Corsi (WalCT) test. In order to assess the effectiveness of mood manipulation, participants filled in the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule before and after carrying out the visuospatial tasks. Firstly, results showed that after mood induction, only the positive affect changed, whereas the negative affect remained unconfounded by mood and by sex. This finding is in line with the main effect of ‘group’ on all tests used: the positive music group scored significantly higher than other groups. Secondly, although men outperformed women in the CBT forward condition and in the WalCT forward and backward conditions, they scored higher than women only in the WalCT with the negative background music. This means that mood cannot fully explain sex differences in visuospatial and navigational working memory. Our results suggest that sex differences in the CBT and WalCT can be better explained by differences in spatial competences rather than by emotional contexts.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Angel LA, Polzella DJ, Elvers GC (2010) Background music and cognitive performance. Percept Mot Skills 11:1059–1064
Astur RS, Ortiz ML, Sutherland RJ (1998) A characterization of performance by men and women in a virtual Morris water task: a large and reliable sex difference. Behav Brain Res 93:185–190
Berch DB, Krikorian R, Huha EM (1998) The corsi blocktapping task: methodological and theoretical considerations. Brain Cogn 38:317–338
Berna C, Leknes S, Holmes EA, Edwards RR, Goodwin GM, Tracey I (2010) Induction of depressed mood disrupts emotion regulation neurocircuitry and enhances pain unpleasantness. Biol Psychiatry 67:1083–1090
Bianchini F, Incoccia C, Palermo L, Piccardi L, Zompanti L, Sabatini U, Peran P, Guariglia C (2010) Developmental topographical disorientation in a healthy subject. Neuropsychologia 48:1563–1573
Bianchini F, Palermo L, Piccardi L, Incoccia C, Nemmi F, Sabatini U, Guariglia C (2014a) Where am I? A new case of developmental topographical disorientation. J Neuropsychol 8:107–124
Bianchini F, Di Vita A, Palermo L, Piccardi L, Blundo C, Guariglia C (2014b) A selective egocentric topographical working memory deficit in the early stages of Alzheimer’s Disease: a preliminary study. Am J Alzheimer’s Disease Other Dement 29:749–754
Bosco A, Longoni AM, Vecchi T (2004) Gender effects in spatial orientation: cognitive profiles and mental strategies. Appl Cogn Psychol 18:519–532
Brown JS, Collins A, Duguid P (1989) Situated cognition and the culture of learning. Educ Res 18:32–42
Capitani E, Laiacona M, Ciceri E (1991) Sex differences in spatial memory: a reanalysis of block-tapping long-term memory according to the short-term memory level. Ital J Neurol Sci 12:461–466
Choi J, McKillop E, L’Hirondelle N, Ward M (2006) Sex-specific relationships between route-learning strategies and abilities in a large-scale environment. Environ Behav 38(6):791–801
Clark A (1997) Being there: putting brain, body, and world together again. MIT Press, Cambridge
Corsi PM (1972) Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain. Dissertation Abstracts International, 34 (02),891B. (UMI No. AA105-77717)
De Goede M, Postma A (2015) Learning your way in a city: experience and gender differences in configurational knowledge of one’s environment. Front Pscyhol 6:402
Eals M, Silverman I (1994) The hunter-gatherer theory of spatial sex differences: proximate factors mediating the female advantage in recall of object arrays. Ethol Sociobiol 15:95–105
Eich E, Metcalfe J (1989) Mood dependent memory for internal versus external events. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 15:443–455
Galea LAM, Kimura D (1993) Sex differences in route learning. Personal Individ Differ 14:53–65
Gerrard-Hesse A, Spies K, Hesse FW (1994) Experimental inductions of emotional states and their effectiveness: a review. Br J Psychol 85:55–78
Gorn G, Pham MT, Sin LY (2001) When arousal influences ad evaluation and valence does not (and vice versa). J Consum Psychol 11:43–55
Halpern DF (1991) Sex differences in cognitive abilities, 2nd edn. Erlbaum, Hillsdale
Husain G, Thompson WF, Schellenberg EG (2002) Effects of musical tempo and mode on arousal, mood, and spatial abilities. Music Percept 20:151–171
Jones MH, West SD, Estell DB (2006) The Mozart effect: arousal, preference, and spatial performance. Psychol Aesthet Creat Arts S(1):26–32
Kämpfe J, Sedlmeier P, Renkewitz F (2011) The impact of background music on adult listeners: a meta-analysis. Psychol Music 39:424–448
Kaufman SB (2007) Sex differences in mental rotation and spatial visualization ability: can they be accounted for by differences in working memory capacity? Intelligence 35:211–223
Kessels RPC, Van Zandvoort MJE, Postma A, Kappelle LJ, De Haan EHF (2000) The Corsi Block-Tapping Task: standardization and normative data. Appl Neuropsychol 7:252–258
Kessels RPC, Postma A, Kappelle LJ, de Haan EHF (2002) Selective impairments in object-location binding, metric encoding and their integration after ischemic stroke. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 24:115–129
Kessels RPC, Van den Berg E, Ruis C, Brands AMA (2008) The backward span of the Corsi Block-Tapping Task and its association with the WAIS-III digit span. Assessment 15:426–434
Kimura D (1999) Sex and cognition. MIT Press, Cambridge
Kliegel M, Jäger T, Phillips LH (2007) Emotional development across adulthood: differential age-related emotional reactivity and emotion regulation in a negative mood induction procedure. Int J Aging Hum Dev 64:217–244
Knight BG, Maines ML, Robinson GS (2002) The effects of sad mood on memory in older adults: a test of the mood congruence effect. Psychol Aging 17:653–661
LaGrone CW (1969) Sex and personality differences in relation to feeling for direction. J Gen Psychol 81:23–33
Lawton CA (1994) Gender differences in way-finding strategies: relationship to spatial ability and spatial anxiety. Sex Roles 30:765–779
Lawton CA (1996) Strategies for indoor wayfinding: the role of orientation. J Environ Psychol 16:137–145
Lawton CA (2010) Gender, spatial abilities, and wayfinding. In: Chrisler JC, McCreary DR (eds) Handbook of gender research in psychology, Vol 1: Gender research in general and experimental psychology. Springer, New York, pp 317–341
Lawton C, Kallai J (2002) Gender differences in wayfinding strategies and anxiety about wayfinding: a cross-cultural comparison. Sex Roles 44:389–401
Linn MC, Petersen AC (1985) Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: a meta-analysis. Child Dev 56:1479–1498
Martin MA, Metha A (1997) Recall of early childhood memories through musical mood induction. Arts Psychother 24:447–454
Masters MS, Sanders B (1993) Is the gender difference in mental rotation disappearing? Behav Genetics 23:337–341
Miller LK, Schyb M (1989) Facilitation and interference by background music. J Music Ther 26:42–54
Monaco M, Costa A, Caltagirone C, Carlesimo GA (2013) Forward and backward span for verbal and visuo-spatial data: standardization and normative data from an Italian adult population. Neurol Sci 34:749–754
Montello DR, Lovelace KL, Golledge RG, Self CM (1999) Sex-related differences and similarities in geographic and environmental spatial abilities. Ann Assoc Am Geogr 80:515–534
Nemmi F, Boccia M, Piccardi L, Galati Guariglia C (2013) Segregation of neural circuits involved in spatial learning in reaching and navigational space. Neuropsychologia 51:1561–1570
Nichelli F, Bulgheroni S, Riva D (2001) Developmental patterns of verbal and visuospatial spans. Neurol Sci 22:377–384
Nori R, Piccardi L (2012) Il senso dell’orientamento: Quanto conta la familiarità con l’ambiente? Giorn It Psicol 39:43–368
Nori R, Piccardi L (2015) I believe I am good at orienting myself…but is that true? Cogn Process 16:301–307
Nori R, Piccardi L, Palermo L, Guariglia C, Giusberti F (2012) Working memory load elicits gender differences in mental imagery. In: McGeown S (ed) Psychology of gender differences. Nova Science Publishers, New York, pp 161–173
Nori R, Piccardi L, Migliori M, Guidazzoli A, Frasca F, De Luca D, Giusberti F (2015a) The virtual reality Walking Corsi Test. Comput Hum Behav 48:72–77
Nori R, Piccardi L, Pelosi A, De Luca D, Frasca F, Giusberti F (2015b) Perspective changing in WalCT and VR-WalCT: a gender difference study [WalCT–VR-WalCT: gender differences]. Comput Hum Behav 53:316–323
Orsini A, Schiappa O, Grossi D (1981) Sex and cultural differences in children’s spatial and verbal memory span. Percept Mot Skills 53:39–42
Orsini A, Chiacchio L, Cinque M, Cocchiaro C, Schiappa O, Grossi D (1986) Effects of age, education, and sex on two tests of immediate memory: a study of normal subjects from 20 to 99 years of age. Percept Mot Skills 63:727–732
Orsini A, Grossi D, Capitani E, Laiacona M, Papagno G, Vallar G (1987) Verbal and spatial immediate memory span: normative data from 1355 to 1112 children. Ital J Neurol Sci 8:539–548
Pagulayan KF, Bush RM, Medina KL, Bartok JA, Krikorian R (2006) Developmental normative data for the Corsi Block-Tapping Task. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 28:1043–1052
Palermo L, Piccardi L, Bianchini F, Nemmi F, Giorgio V, Incoccia C, Sabatini U, Guariglia C (2014) Looking for the compass in a case of developmental topographical disorientation: a behavioural and neuroimaging study. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 36:464–481
Palmiero M, Borsellino MC (2014) Embodied cognition: Comprendere la mente incarnata. ARAS Edizioni, Fano (Pu)
Palmiero M, Nori R, Rogolino C, D’Amico S, Piccardi L (2015) Situated navigational working memory: the role of positive mood. Cogn Process 16:327–330
Perdue BC, Summers JO (1986) Checking the success of manipulations in marketing experiments. J Mark Res 23:317–326
Piccardi L, Nori R (2011) Effects of visuo-spatial working memory on wayfinding ability. In: Levin ES (ed) Working memory: capacity, developments and improvement techniques. Nova Science, New York, pp 81–108
Piccardi L, Iaria G, Ricci M, Bianchini F, Zompanti L, Guariglia C (2008) Walking in the Corsi test: which type of memory do you need? Neurosci Lett 432:127–131
Piccardi L, Berthoz A, Baulac M, Denos M, Dupont S, Samson S, Guariglia C (2010) Different spatial memory systems are involved in small and large-scale environments: evidence from patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Exp Brain Res 206:171–177
Piccardi L, Iaria G, Bianchini F, Zompanti L, Guariglia C (2011a) Dissociated deficits of visuo-spatial memory in near space and navigational space: evidence from brain-damaged patients and healthy older participants. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 18:362–384
Piccardi L, Risetti M, Nori R, Tanzilli A, Bernardi L, Guariglia C (2011b) Perspecitive changing in primary and secondary learning: a gender difference study. Learn Individ Differ 21:114–118
Piccardi L, Bianchini F, Argento O, De Nigris A, Maialetti A, Palermo L, Guariglia C (2013) The Walking Corsi Test (WalCT): standardization of the topographical memory test in an Italian population. Neurol Sci 34:971–978
Piccardi L, Bianchini F, Nori R, Marano A, Iachini F, Lasala L, Guariglia C (2014a) Spatial location and pathway memory compared in the reaching versus walking domains. Neurosci Lett 566:226–230
Piccardi L, Leonzi M, D’Amico S, Marano A, Guariglia C (2014b) Development of navigational working memory: evidence from 6- to 10-year-old children. Br J Dev Psychol 32:205–217
Piccardi L, Palermo L, Leonzi M, Risetti M, Zompanti L, D’Amico S, Guariglia C (2014c) The Walking Corsi Test (WalCT): a normative study of topographical working memory in a sample of 4- to 11-year-olds. Clin Neuropsychol 28:84–96
Piccardi L, Nori R, Boccia M, Barbetti S, Verde P, Guariglia C, Ferlazzo F (2015) A dedicated system for topographical working memory: evidence from specific-domain task interference. Exp Brain Res 233:2489–2495
Picucci L, Caffò AO, Bosco A (2011) Besides navigation accuracy: gender differences in strategy selection and level of spatial confidence. J Environ Psychol 31:430–438
Rauscher FH, Shaw GL, Ky KN (1993) Music and spatial task performance. Nature 365:611
Riener CR, Stefanucci JK, Proffitt DR, Clore G (2011) An effect of mood on the perception of geographical slant. Cogn Emot 25:174–182
Sarason IG (1980) Test anxiety: theory, research, and applications. Erlbaum, Hillsdale
Saucier DM, McCreary DR, Saxberg JKJ (2002) Does gender role socialization mediate sex differences in mental rotations? Personal Individ Differ 32:1101–1111
Schellenberg EG (2005) Music and cognitive abilities. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 14:317–320
Schwartz N (2002) Situated cognition and the wisdom of feelings: cognitive tuning. In: Barrett LF, Salovey P (eds) The wisdom in feeling. Guilford Press, New York, pp 144–166
Schwarz N, Clore GL (1996) Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In: Higgins ET, Kruglanski A (eds) Social psychology: handbook of basic principles. Guilford, New York, pp 433–465
Shah DS, Prados J, Gamble J, De Lillo C, Gibson CL (2013) Sex differences in spatial memory using serial and search tasks. Behav Brain Res 257:90–99
Silverman I, Choi J, Mackewn A, Fisher M, Moro J, Olshansky E (2000) Evolved mechanisms underlying wayfinding: further studies on the hunter-gatherer theory of spatial sex differences. Evolut Hum Behav 21:201–213
Silverman I, Choi J, Peters M (2007) The hunter-gatherer theory of sex differences in spatial abilities: data from 40 countries. Arch Sex Behav 36(2):261–268
Sloboda JA, Juslin PN (2001) Psychological perspectives on music and emotion. In: Juslin PN, Sloboda JA (eds) Music and emotion: theory and research. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 71–104
Smirni P, Villardita C, Zappala G (1983) Spatial memory span in adolescents: cultural and sex differences. Percept Mot Skills 57:175–178
Spinnler H, Tognoni G (1987) Standardizzazione e taratura italiana di test neuropsicologici. Ital J Neurol Sci 8:1–120
Terracciano A, McCrae RR, Costa PT Jr (2003) Factorial and construct validity of the Italian positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS). Eur J Psychol Assess 19:131–141
Thompson WF, Schellenberg EG, Husain G (2001) Arousal, mood and the Mozart effect. Psychol Sci 12:248–251
Verde P, Piccardi L, Bianchini F, Trivelloni P, Gariglia C, Tomao E (2013) Gender effects on mental rotation in pilots versus nonpilots. Aviat Space Env Med 84:726–729
Voyer D, Voyer S, Bryden MP (1995) Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: a meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychol Bull 117:250–270
Wilson M (2002) Six views of embodied cognition. Psychon Bull Rev 9:625–636
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Palmiero, M., Nori, R., Rogolino, C. et al. Sex differences in visuospatial and navigational working memory: the role of mood induced by background music. Exp Brain Res 234, 2381–2389 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4643-3
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4643-3