[go: up one dir, main page]
More Web Proxy on the site http://driver.im/ Skip to main content
Log in

Perception of 3-D location based on vision, touch, and extended touch

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Perception of the near environment gives rise to spatial images in working memory that continue to represent the spatial layout even after cessation of sensory input. As the observer moves, these spatial images are continuously updated. This research is concerned with (1) whether spatial images of targets are formed when they are sensed using extended touch (i.e., using a probe to extend the reach of the arm) and (2) the accuracy with which such targets are perceived. In Experiment 1, participants perceived the 3-D locations of individual targets from a fixed origin and were then tested with an updating task involving blindfolded walking followed by placement of the hand at the remembered target location. Twenty-four target locations, representing all combinations of two distances, two heights, and six azimuths, were perceived by vision or by blindfolded exploration with the bare hand, a 1-m probe, or a 2-m probe. Systematic errors in azimuth were observed for all targets, reflecting errors in representing the target locations and updating. Overall, updating after visual perception was best, but the quantitative differences between conditions were small. Experiment 2 demonstrated that auditory information signifying contact with the target was not a factor. Overall, the results indicate that 3-D spatial images can be formed of targets sensed by extended touch and that perception by extended touch, even out to 1.75 m, is surprisingly accurate.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
£29.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price includes VAT (United Kingdom)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Practice with blind walking is not usually provided in studies using blind walking or blind walking/gesturing techniques to assess distance perception. Even without it, research has shown that the mean indicated distances using blind walking to visual targets viewed with full distance cues are very accurate (for a summary, see Loomis and Philbeck 2008). The ability to perform blind walking and blind walking/gesturing must require some calibration of the overall scaling factor for walked distance. Normally, this calibration surely is provided by observing the correspondence between walking speed and observed changes in the visual scene during normal ambulation (Loomis and Philbeck 2008, p. 20). Even with such calibration, large systematic performance errors are readily apparent when distance cues are impoverished, errors that are traceable to errors in perceiving distance (for summary, see Loomis and Philbeck 2008). However, because we wished to minimize errors in perceived displacements associated with blind walking, we chose to provide practice and feedback with only this component of the response. Thus, the practice served only to precisely calibrate perceived displacements associated with walking.

References

  • Barac-Cikoja D, Turvey MT (1991) Perceiving aperture size by striking. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 17:330–346

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Barber PO, Lederman SJ (1988) Encoding direction in manipulatory space and the role of visual experience. J Vis Impair Blindness 82:99–106

    Google Scholar 

  • Cabe PA, Wright CD, Wright MA (2003) Descartes’s blind man revisited: bimanual triangulation of distance using static hand-held rods. Am J Psychol 116:71–98

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cardinali L, Frassinetti F, Brozzoli C, Urquizar C, Roy AC, Farne A (2009) Tool-use induces morphological updating of the body schema. Curr Biol 19:R478–R479

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carello C, Fitzpatrick P, Turvey MT (1992) Haptic probing: perceiving the length of a probe and the distance of a surface probed. Percept Psychophys 51:580–598

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chan TC, Turvey MT (1991) The vertical distances of surfaces by means of a hand-held probe. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 17:347–358

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein W, Hughes B, Schneider SL, Bach-y-Rita P (1986) Is there anything out there? A study of distal attribution in response to vibrotactile stimulation. Perception 15:275–284

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson JJ (1966) The senses considered as perceptual systems. Houghton-Mifflin, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Giudice NA, Klatzky RL, Loomis JM (2009) Evidence for amodal representations after bimodal learning: integration of haptic-visual layouts into a common spatial image. Spatial Cogn Comput 9:287–304

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giudice NA, Betty MR, Loomis JM (2011) Functional equivalence of spatial images from touch and vision: evidence from spatial updating in blind and sighted individuals. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 37:621–634

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hollins M, Kelley EK (1988) Spatial updating in blind and sighted people. Percept Psychophys 43:380–388

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes NP, Spence C (2004) The body schema and the multisensory representation(s) of peripersonal space. Cogn Process 5:94–105

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horn DL, Loomis JM (2004) Spatial updating of targets in front and behind. Paidéia 14:75–81

    Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher J, Newcombe N, Sandberg E (1994) The coding of spatial location in young children. Cogn Psychol 27:115–147

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Iriki A, Tanaka M, Iwamura Y (1996) Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. NeuroReport 7:2325–2330

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Katz D (1989) The world of touch. (trans: Krueger LE). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates (Originally published, 1925). Hillsdale, NJ

  • Klatzky RL, Lippa Y, Loomis JM, Golledge RG (2003) Encoding, learning, and spatial updating of multiple object locations specified by 3-D sound, spatial language, and vision. Exp Brain Res 149:48–61

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Li Z, Durgin FH (2012) A comparison of two theories of perceived distance on the ground plane: the angular expansion hypothesis and the intrinsic bias hypothesis. iPerception 3:368–383

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loomis JM (1992) Distal attribution and presence. Presence Teleoper Virtual Environ 1:113–119

    Google Scholar 

  • Loomis JM, Philbeck JW (2008) Spatial perception with spatial updating and action. In: Klatzky RL, Behrmann M, MacWhinney B (eds) Embodiment, ego-space, and action. Taylor and Francis, New York, pp 1–43

    Google Scholar 

  • Loomis JM, Lippa Y, Golledge RG, Klatzky RL (2002) Spatial updating of locations specified by 3-d sound and spatial language. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 28:335–345

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loomis JM, Klatzky RL, McHugh B, Giudice NA (2012) Spatial working memory for 3-D locations specified by vision and audition. Attent Percept Psychophys 74:1260–1267

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loomis JM, Klatzky RL, Giudice NA (in press) Representing 3D space in working memory: Spatial images from vision, touch, hearing, and language. In: Lacey S, Lawson R (eds) Multisensory imagery: theory and applications. Springer, New York

  • Lotze H (1894) Microcosmus: an essay concerning man and his relation to the world (trans: Hamilton E, Jones EEC). In, vol I, Book V. T and T Clark Edinburgh, p 588

  • Maravita A, Iriki A (2004) Tools for the body (schema). Trends Cogn Sci 8:79–86

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • May M, Vogeley K (2006) Remembering where on the basis of spatial action and spatial language. Paper presented at the International Workshop on Knowing that and Knowing how in Space, Bonn, Germany

    Google Scholar 

  • Ooi TL, He ZJ (2006) Localizing suspended objects in the intermediate distance range (>2 meters) by observers with normal and abnormal binocular vision. J Vis 6:422a

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ooi TL, Wu B, He ZJ (2001) Distance determined by the angular declination below the horizon. Nature 414:197–200

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ooi TL, Wu B, He ZJ (2006) Perceptual space in the dark affected by the intrinsic bias of the visual system. Perception 35:605–624

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pasqualotto A, Finucane CM, Newell FN (2005) Visual and haptic representations of scenes are updated with observer movement. Exp Brain Res 166:481–488

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Philbeck JW, Loomis JM (1997) Comparison of two indicators of visually perceived egocentric distance under full-cue and reduced-cue conditions. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 23:72–85

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Philbeck JW, Loomis JM, Beall AC (1997) Visually perceived location is an invariant in the control of action. Percept Psychophys 59:601–612

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Polanyi M (1966) The tacit dimension. Doubleday, Garden City, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Povinelli DJ, Reaux JE, Frey SH (2011) Chimpanzees’ context-dependent tool use provides evidence for separable representations of hand and tool even during active use within peripersonal space. Neuropsychologia 48:243–247

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegle JH, Warren WH (2010) Distal attribution and distance perception in sensory substitution. Perception 39:208–223

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Speigle JM, Loomis JM (1993) Auditory distance perception by translating observers. In: Proceedings of IEEE symposium on research frontiers in virtual reality, San Jose, CA, October 25–26, 1993

  • Turvey MT (1996) Dynamic touch. Am Psychol 51:1134–1152

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Weber EH (1978) Der Tastsinn (trans: Murray DJ). Academic Press (Originally published, 1846). New York

  • Wu B, Ooi TL, He ZJ (2004) Perceiving distances accurately by a directional process of integrating ground information. Nature 428:73–77

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Yamamoto N, Shelton AL (2005) Visual and proprioceptive representations in spatial memory. Mem Cogn 33:140–150

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yamamoto N, Shelton AL (2007) Path information effects in visual and proprioceptive spatial learning. Acta Psychol 125:346–360

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was funded by NIH grant 1R01EY016817. The authors thank Mick Ramos for assistance in recruiting and running participants and Tim McGrath for assistance with coding the experimental scripts.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nicholas A. Giudice.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Giudice, N.A., Klatzky, R.L., Bennett, C.R. et al. Perception of 3-D location based on vision, touch, and extended touch. Exp Brain Res 224, 141–153 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3295-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3295-1

Keywords