Abstract
The automotive market today has seen the entry of Level-3 conditional automated driving vehicles equipped with an automated driving system that waits for the drivers to start it on the road. Before making a full assessment of the use of automated driving systems, drivers should be made to experience real-world conditional automated driving. A driver may have a mood change when driving a real-world automated vehicle. This emotion points to the mediation of motivation, which affects a driver’s cognition and intention to start an automated driving system on the road. In this study, the emotion of experiencing autonomous driving, cognition, and satisfaction of the driving performance were introduced to construct an intention model to start an automated driving system. Online and off-line questionnaires were adopted, and the emotional response, cognition of automated driving, and intention of 133 drivers who experienced real-world conditional automated driving were determined. Driver experience was assessed in four scenarios as part of emotional tests: during manual driving, during conditional automated driving, during takeover under the influence of the warning system, and during takeover driving. The results of the questionnaire showed a significant positive correlation between emotion and cognition, satisfaction of autonomous driving performance, and the intent to start the automated driving system. Emotions play a mediating role between cognition, satisfaction, and intention to start automated driving. Drivers who experienced conditional automated driving appeared to exhibit a moderately high level of emotional response in terms of joy, interest, and surprise, whereas medium-level negative emotions included fear and anger. Drivers experienced some intensity of emotional changes during conditional automated driving and takeover driving. The emotional changes were uneven but encouraging support was reported. In addition, specific hypotheses relating the driving performance of the automated vehicles (in terms of programmed design of takeover and warning system of takeover) to the emotional dimensions were tested. A cluster analysis of the emotional response measures revealed five different emotional patterns when experiencing the real-world automated vehicle, among which the happy/satisfied group had higher intention to start an automated driving system on the road, followed by the emotional group, whereas the disgust group showed the lowest intention. The cluster analysis was supported by demographic and driving cognitive characteristics (age, education, and self-evaluation of the driving level and driving experience) of the five groups of drivers. Finally, the theoretical and practical significance of this study was expounded. The research results may provide some suggestions and hints for the government and enterprises to promote the development of automated driving.
Similar content being viewed by others
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.References
Abbink DA, Mulder M, Boer ER (2012) Haptic shared control: smoothly shifting control authority? Cogn Technol Work 14 (1): 19–28. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10111-011-0192-5
Aeberhard M, Rauch S, Bahram M, Tanzmeister G, Thomas J, Pilat Y, Kaempchen N (2015) Experience, results and lessons learned from automated driving on Germany’s highways. IEEE Intell Transp Syst Mag 7 (1): 42–57
Alambeigi H, McDonald AD (2021) A Bayesian regression analysis of the effects of alert presence and scenario criticality on automated vehicle takeover performance. Hum Fact 2: 2
Alexandre B, Reynaud E, Osiurak F, Navarro J (2018) Acceptance and acceptability criteria: a literature review. Cogn Technol Work 20 (2): 165–177
Ball K, Owsley C, Sloane ME, Roenker DL, Bruni JR (1993) Visual attention problems as a predictor of vehicle crashes in older drivers. Invest Ophthalmol vis Sci 34 (11): 3110–3123
Banks VA, Plant KL, Stanton NA (2018) Driver error or designer error: using the perceptual cycle model to explore the circumstances surrounding the fatal tesla crash on 7th May 2016. Saf Sci 108: 278–285
Bayer JB, Campbell SW (2012) Texting while driving on automatic: considering the frequency-independent side of habit. Comput Hum Behav 28 (6): 2083–2090
Becheur I, Guizani H, Shaaban K (2019) Belief in fate and self-efficacy in road safety advertising based on guilt: an explanation based on negotiable fate. Austr Market J (AMJ) 27 (4): 233–241
Berger U, Anaki D (2014) Demographic influences on disgust: evidence from a heterogeneous sample. Personality Individ Differ 64: 67–71
Borojeni SS, Meschtscherjakov A, Mirnig AG, Boll S, Naujoks F, Politis I, & Alverez I (2017) Control transition workshop: Handover and takeover procedures in highly automated driving. In proceedings of the 9th international conference on automotive user interfaces and interactive vehicular applications adjunct (pp. 39–46).
Brave S, Nass C (2009) Emotion in human-computer interaction. Hum-Comput Interact Fundam 20094635 (53–68): 4
Brown JS, Farber IE (1951) Emotions conceptualized as intervening variables—with suggestions toward a theory of frustration. Psychol Bull 48 (6): 465
Camras L (1980) Emotion: a psychoevolutionary synthesis.
Carsten O, Martens MH (2019) How can humans understand their automated cars? HMI principles, problems and solutions. Cogn Technol Work 21 (1): 3–20
Celebi ME, Kingravi HA, Vela PA (2013) A comparative study of efficient initialization methods for the k-means clustering algorithm. Expert Syst Appl 40 (1): 200–210
Chee PNE, Susilo YO, Wong YD (2021) Longitudinal interactions between experienced users’ service valuations and willingness-to-use a first-/last-mile automated bus service. Travel Behav Soc 22: 252–261
Chikaraishi M, Khan D, Yasuda B, Fujiwara A (2020) Risk perception and social acceptability of autonomous vehicles: a case study in Hiroshima, Japan. Transp Policy 98: 105–115
China Association of Automobile Manufactures (2020a) Top design of intelligent vehicle has been launched and trillion scale market is waiting for exploring. http: //www.caam.org.cn/chn/38/cate_421/con_5228850.html. Accessed 27 February 2020a (in Chinese)
China Association of Automobile Manufactures (2020b) Research and application of automatic driving show new development prospects. http: //www.caam.org.cn/chn/38/cate_421/con_5231345.html. Accessed 5 August 2020b (in Chinese)
Darley WK, Lim JS (1992) The effect of consumers’ emotional reactions on behavioral intention: The moderating role of personal relevance and self-monitoring. Psychol Mark 9 (4): 329–346
De Craen S, Twisk DA, Hagenzieker MP, Elffers H, Brookhuis KA (2011) Do young novice drivers overestimate their driving skills more than experienced drivers? Different methods lead to different conclusions. Accid Anal Prev 43 (5): 1660–1665
Dixon G, Hart PS, Clarke C, O’Donnell NH, Hmielowski J (2020) What drives support for self-driving car technology in the United States? J Risk Res 23 (3): 275–287
Du N, Yang XJ, Zhou F (2020a) Psychophysiological responses to takeover requests in conditionally automated driving. Accid Anal Prev 148: 105804
Du N, Zhou F, Pulver EM, Tilbury DM, Robert LP, Pradhan AK, Yang XJ (2020b) Examining the effects of emotional valence and arousal on takeover performance in conditionally automated driving. Transp Res Part C Emerg Technol 112: 78–87
Dube L, Belanger MC, Trudeau E (1996) The role of emotions in health care satisfaction. Mark Health Serv 16 (2): 45
Ekman P (1992) An argument for basic emotions. Cogn Emot 6 (3–4): 169–200
Ellsworth PC (1994) Some reasons to expect universal antecedents of emotion. The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions, 150–154.
Fagnant DJ, Kockelman K (2015) Preparing a nation for autonomous vehicles: opportunities, barriers and policy recommendations. Transp Res Part A Policy Pract 77: 167–181
Fischer KW, Shaver PR, Carnochan P (1990) How emotions develop and how they organise development. Cogn Emot 4 (2): 81–127
Gardner GT, Gould LC (1989) Public perceptions of the risks and benefits of technology 1. Risk Anal 9 (2): 225–242
Gauld CS, Lewis I, White KM (2014) Concealed texting while driving: What are young people’s beliefs about this risky behaviour? Saf Sci 65: 63–69
Gold C, Körber M, Hohenberger C, Lechner D, Bengler K (2015) Trust in automation–before and after the experience of take-over scenarios in a highly automated vehicle. Proc Manuf 3: 3025–3032
Grillon C, Lago T, Stahl S, Beale A, Balderston N, Ernst M (2020) Better cognitive efficiency is associated with increased experimental anxiety. Psychophysiology 57 (8): e13559
Guo X, Wang Z (2007) Concept, function and meaning of positive emotion. Adv Psychol Sci 15 (05): 810
Harmon-Jones E (2004) Contributions from research on anger and cognitive dissonance to understanding the motivational functions of asymmetrical frontal brain activity. Biol Psychol 67 (1–2): 51–76
Hohenberger C, Spörrle M, Welpe IM (2017) Not fearless, but self-enhanced: The effects of anxiety on the willingness to use autonomous cars depend on individual levels of self-enhancement. Technol Forecast Soc Chang 116: 40–52
Holden RJ (2011) Cognitive performance-altering effects of electronic medical records: an application of the human factors paradigm for patient safety. Cogn Technol Work 13 (1): 11–29
Hu TY, Xie X, Li J (2013) Negative or positive? The effect of emotion and mood on risky driving. Transport Res F: Traffic Psychol Behav 16: 29–40
Huang G, Petersen C, Pitts BJ (2020). The Impact of Mental States on Semi-autonomous Driving Takeover Performance: A Systematic Review. In Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting (Vol. 64, No. 1, pp. 1372–1376). Sage CA: Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications.
Hutcherson CA, Gross JJ (2011) The moral emotions: a social–functionalist account of anger, disgust, and contempt. J Pers Soc Psychol 100 (4): 719
Inagaki T, Sheridan TB (2019) A critique of the SAE conditional driving automation definition, and analyses of options for improvement. Cogn Technol Work 21 (4): 569–578
Izard CE (1977) Differential emotions theory. Human emotions. Springer, Boston, pp 43–66
Izard CE (2011) Forms and functions of emotions: matters of emotion–cognition interactions. Emot Rev 3 (4): 371–378
James W (1884) What is an emotion? Mind 9: 188–205
Kaleńska-Rodzaj J (2020) Music performance anxiety and pre-performance emotions in the light of psychology of emotion and emotion regulation. Psychol Music. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735620961154
Kim H, Song M, Doerzaph Z (2020) Real-world use of partially automated driving systems and driver impressions. In Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting (Vol. 64, No. 1, pp. 1092–1093). Los Angeles: SAGE Publications.
Kohl C, Knigge M, Baader G, Böhm M, Krcmar H (2018) Anticipating acceptance of emerging technologies using twitter: the case of self-driving cars. J Bus Econ 88 (5): 617–642
Koo J, Kwac J, Ju W, Steinert M, Leifer L, Nass C (2015) Why did my car just do that? Explaining semi-autonomous driving actions to improve driver understanding, trust, and performance. Int J Interact Des Manuf (IJIDeM) 9 (4): 269–275
Kyriakidis M, Happee R, de Winter JC (2015) Public opinion on automated driving: results of an international questionnaire among 5000 respondents. Transp Res Traffic Psychol Behav 32: 127–140
Lai WT, Chen CF (2011) Behavioral intentions of public transit passengers—The roles of service quality, perceived value, satisfaction and involvement. Transp Policy 18 (2): 318–325
Lajunen T, Summala H (1995) Driving experience, personality, and skill and safety-motive dimensions in drivers’ self-assessments. Person Individ Differ 19 (3): 307–318
Lange CG (1885) The mechanism of the emotions. Classic Psychol 2: 672–684
LeDoux JE (2014) Coming to terms with fear. Proc Natl Acad Sci 111 (8): 2871–2878
Lee YJ (2016) The relationships amongst emotional experience, cognition, and behavioural intention in battlefield tourism. Asia Pac J Tour Res 21 (6): 697–715
Li S, Blythe P, Guo W, Namdeo A (2019) Investigation of older drivers’ requirements of the human-machine interaction in highly automated vehicles. Transport Res F: Traffic Psychol Behav 62: 546–563
Linnenbrink EA, Pintrich PR (2004) Role of affect in cognitive processing in academic contexts. Motivation, emotion, and cognition: Integrative perspectives on intellectual functioning and development, 57–87.
Liu H, Yang R, Wang L, Liu P (2019a) Evaluating initial public acceptance of highly and fully autonomous vehicles. Int J Hum-Comput Interact 35 (11): 919–931
Liu P, Xu Z, Zhao X (2019b) Road tests of self-driving vehicles: affective and cognitive pathways in acceptance formation. Transp Res Part A Policy Pract 124: 354–369
Liu P, Jiang Z, Li T, Wang G, Wang R, Xu Z (2021) User experience and usability when the automated driving system fails: findings from a field experiment. Accid Anal Prev 161: 106383
Martin D, O’neill M, Hubbard S, Palmer A, (2008) The role of emotion in explaining consumer satisfaction and future behavioural intention. J Serv Market 2: 1
McNeely LE, Burton JL, Pennington JR (2020) Emotional and cognitive drivers of customer satisfaction in adventure travel. Tour Rev Int 23 (3–4): 133–147
Merat N, Jamson AH, Lai FC, Carsten O (2012) Highly automated driving, secondary task performance, and driver state. Hum Fact 54 (5): 762–771
Merat N, Seppelt B, Louw T, Engström J, Lee JD, Johansson E, Keinath A (2019) The “out-of-the-loop” concept in automated driving: proposed definition, measures and implications. Cogn Technol Work 21 (1): 87–98
Mesken J, Hagenzieker MP, Rothengatter T, De Waard D (2007) Frequency, determinants, and consequences of different drivers’ emotions: an on-the-road study using self-reports, (observed) behaviour, and physiology. Transp Res Traffic Psychol Behav 10 (6): 458–475
Moon MA, Khalid MJ, Awan HM, Attiq S, Rasool H, Kiran M (2017) Consumer’s perceptions of website’s utilitarian and hedonic attributes and online purchase intentions: a cognitive–affective attitude approach. Span J Market-ESIC 21 (2): 73–88
Moons I, De Pelsmacker P (2012) Emotions as determinants of electric car usage intention. J Mark Manag 28 (3–4): 195–237
Moors A, Ellsworth PC, Scherer KR, Frijda NH (2013) Appraisal theories of emotion: state of the art and future development. Emot Rev 5 (2): 119–124
Moreno AT, Michalski A, Llorca C, Moeckel R (2018) Shared autonomous vehicles effect on vehicle-km traveled and average trip duration. J Adv Transp. https://doi.org/10.1155/2018/8969353
Nass C, Jonsson IM, Harris H, Reaves B, Endo J, Brave S, Takayama L (2005). Improving automotive safety by pairing driver emotion and car voice emotion. In CHI'05 extended abstracts on Human factors in computing systems (pp. 1973–1976). DOI: https://doi.org/10.1145/1056808.1057070
Naujoks F, Purucker C, Wiedemann K, Marberger C (2019) Noncritical state transitions during conditionally automated driving on german freeways: effects of non–driving related tasks on takeover time and takeover quality. Hum Factors 61 (4): 596–613
Naujoks F, Mai C, and Neukum A (2014) The effect of urgency of take-over requests during highly automated driving under distraction conditions. Advances in human aspects of transportation, 7 (Part I), 431.
Naujoks F, Wiedemann K, Schömig N (2017). The importance of interruption management for usefulness and acceptance of automated driving. In Proceedings of the 9th international conference on automotive user interfaces and interactive vehicular applications (pp. 254–263).
Nees MA (2019) Safer than the average human driver (who is less safe than me)? Examining a popular safety benchmark for self-driving cars. J Safety Res 69: 61–68
Netzer L, Igra L, Anan YB, Tamir M (2015) When bad emotions seem better: experience changes the automatic evaluation of anger. Soc Psychol Personal Sci 6 (7): 797–804
Neumann PG (2016) Automated car woes–-whoa there! Ubiquity 2016 (July): 1–6
Nezu AM, Nezu CM, Peacock MA, Girdwood CP (2004) Case formulation in cognitive-behavior therapy. In: SN Haynes, EM Heiby (Eds) Comprehensive handbook of psychological assessment, Vol. 3. Behavioral assessment (p. 402–426). John Wiley & Sons, Inc
Noel L, de Rubens GZ, Sovacool BK, Kester J (2019) Fear and loathing of electric vehicles: the reactionary rhetoric of range anxiety. Energy Res Soc Sci 48: 96–107
Noordewier MK, Topolinski S, Van Dijk E (2016) The temporal dynamics of surprise. Soc Pers Psychol Compass 10 (3): 136–149
Oliver RL (2014) Satisfaction: a behavioral perspective on the consumer: A behavioral perspective on the consumer. Routledge, New York
Oliver RL, DeSARBO WS (1989) Processing of the satisfaction response in consumption: a suggested framework and research propositions. J Consum Satisf Dissatisf Complain Behav 2 (1): 1–16
Oliver R, Westbrook R (1993) Profiles of consumer emotions and satisfaction in ownership and usage. Emotion 6 (13): 12–27
Omozik K, Yang Y, Kuntermann I, Hergeth S, and Bengler K (2019). How long does it take to relax? Observation of driver behavior during real-world conditionally automated driving. https://ir.uiowa.edu/drivingassessment/2019/papers/37/, Iowa Research Online, 2019 Driving Assessment Conference, doi: https://doi.org/10.17077/drivingassessment.1702.
Parnell KJ, Stanton NA, Plant KL (2018) Good intentions: drivers’ decisions to engage with technology on the road and in a driving simulator. Cogn Technol Work 20 (4): 597–619
Pearl TH (2017) Fast & furious: the misregulation of driverless cars. NYU Ann Surv Am l 73: 19
Pêcher C, Lemercier C, Cellier JM (2009) The influence of emotions on driving behaviour. Hindawi Publishers, New-York
Peters E, Västfjäll D, Gärling T, Slovic P (2006) Affect and decision making: A “hot” topic. J Behav Decis Mak 19 (2): 79–85
Pink S, Osz K, Raats K, Lindgren T, Fors V (2020) Design anthropology for emerging technologies: trust and sharing in autonomous driving futures. Des Stud 69: 100942
Raudenbush B, Frank RA (1999) Assessing food neophobia: the role of stimulus familiarity. Appetite 32 (2): 261–271
Raue M, D’Ambrosio LA, Ward C, Lee C, Jacquillat C, Coughlin JF (2019) The influence of feelings while driving regular cars on the perception and acceptance of self-driving cars. Risk Anal 39 (2): 358–374
Robertson RD, Meister SR, Vanlaar WG, Hing MM (2017) Automated vehicles and behavioural adaptation in Canada. Transp Res Part A Policy Practice 104: 50–57
Robins S, Novaco RW (1999) Systems conceptualization and treatment of anger. J Clin Psychol 55 (3): 325–337
Rogers EM (2010) Diffusion of innovations. Simon and Schuster
Rolls ET (1990) A theory of emotion, and its application to understanding the neural basis of emotion. Cogn Emot 4 (3): 161–190
Rosaldo MZ (1983) The shame of headhunters and the autonomy of self. Ethos 11 (3): 135–151
Rubin DC, Berntsen D (2003) Life scripts help to maintain autobiographical memories of highly positive, but not highly negative, events. Mem Cognit 31 (1): 1–14
Russell JA (1979) Affective space is bipolar. J Pers Soc Psychol 37 (3): 345
Saariluoma P (2020) User psychology of emotional interaction—usability, user experience and technology ethics. Emotions in technology design: From experience to ethics. Springer, Cham, pp 15–26
Sanghavi H, Zhang Y, Jeon M (2020) Effects of anger and display urgency on takeover performance in semi-automated vehicles. In 12th International Conference on Automotive User Interfaces and Interactive Vehicular Applications (pp. 48–56).
Schneemann F, Diederichs F (2019) Action prediction with the Jordan model of human intention: a contribution to cooperative control. Cogn Technol Work 21 (4): 711–721
Schraagen JM, Elsasser P, Fricke H, Hof M, Ragalmuto F (2020) Trusting the X in XAI: Effects of different types of explanations by a self-driving car on trust, explanation satisfaction and mental models. In Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting (Vol. 64, No. 1, pp. 339–343). Sage CA: Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications.
Schwarz C, Gaspar J, Brown T (2019) The effect of reliability on drivers’ trust and behavior in conditional automation. Cogn Technol Work 21 (1): 41–54
Siegrist M (2000) The influence of trust and perceptions of risks and benefits on the acceptance of gene technology. Risk Anal 20 (2): 195–204
Sokolowska J, Sleboda P (2015) The inverse relation between risks and benefits: the role of affect and expertise. Risk Anal 35 (7): 1252–1267
Sullivan JM, Flannagan MJ, Pradhan AK, Bao S (2016) Literature review of behavioral adaptations to advanced driver assistance systems. https://aaafoundation.org/wp-content/uploads/2017/12/BehavioralAdaptationADAS.pdf
Taarvig E, Solbakken OA, Grova B, Monsen JT (2016) Exploring affect integration in children with anxiety disorders: a pilot study. Psychol Conscious Theory Res Pract 3 (4): 338
Tang F, Gao F, Wang Z (2020) Driving capability-based transition strategy for cooperative driving: from manual to automatic. IEEE Access 8: 139013–139022
Taylor J, Deane F, Podd J (2002) Driving-related fear: a review. Clin Psychol Rev 22 (5): 631–645
Techer F, Ojeda L, Barat D, Marteau JY, Rampillon F, Feron S, Dogan E (2019b) Anger and highly automated driving in urban areas: the role of time pressure. Transport Res Traffic Psychol Behav 64: 353–360
Techer F, Dogan E, Rampillon F, Ojeda L, Feron S, Barat D, Marteau JY (2019a) Providing contextual information when encountering traffic interruptions during automated driving: a preliminary study. In Proceedings of the 11th International Conference on Automotive User Interfaces and Interactive Vehicular Applications: Adjunct Proceedings (pp. 456–460)
Trevarthen C (1977) Cognition and reality: principles and implications of cognitive psychology
Turner JH (2009) The sociology of emotions: basic theoretical arguments. Emot Rev 1 (4): 340–354
Vanderhaegen F (2010) Human-error-based design of barriers and analysis of their uses. Cogn Technol Work 12 (2): 133–142
Vanderhaegen F (2012) Cooperation and learning to increase the autonomy of ADAS. Cogn Technol Work 14 (1): 61–69
Vanderhaegen F (2016) Toward a petri net based model to control conflicts of autonomy between cyber-physical & human-systems. IFAC-PapersOnLine 49 (32): 36–41
Vanderhaegen F (2017) Towards increased systems resilience: New challenges based on dissonance control for human reliability in cyber-physical&human systems. Annu Rev Control 44: 316–322
Vanderhaegen F (2021a) Heuristic-based method for conflict discovery of shared control between humans and autonomous systems-A driving automation case study. Robot Auton Syst 146: 103867
Vanderhaegen F (2021b) Weak signal-oriented investigation of ethical dissonance applied to unsuccessful mobility experiences linked to human–machine interactions. Sci Eng Ethics 27 (1): 1–25
Vanderhaegen F, Carsten O (2017) Can dissonance engineering improve risk analysis of human–machine systems? Cogn Technol Work 19 (1): 1–12
Venkatesh V, Morris MG, Davis GB, Davis FD (2003) User acceptance of information technology: toward a unified view. MIS Q 27 (3): 425–478
Visschers VH, Siegrist M (2018) Differences in risk perception between hazards and between individuals. Psychological perspectives on risk and risk analysis. Springer, Cham, pp 63–80
Wang X (2014) The role of empathic concern, internalized moral principle, and anticipated guilt in college students’ intentions to send or read text messages while driving. Int J Health Promot Educ 52 (6): 346–354
Wegner DM, Bargh JA (1998) Control and automaticity in social life. The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 1). McGraw-Hill, Boston.
Westbrook RA (1987) Product/consumption-based affective responses and postpurchase processes. J Mark Res 24 (3): 258–270
Westbrook RA, Oliver RL (1991) The dimensionality of consumption emotion patterns and consumer satisfaction. J Consum Res 18 (1): 84–91
Wintersberger P, Riener A, Frison AK (2016) Automated driving system, male, or female driver: Who'd you prefer? comparative analysis of passengers' mental conditions, emotional states & qualitative feedback. In Proceedings of the 8th international conference on automotive user interfaces and interactive vehicular applications (pp. 51–58).
Wulvik AS, Dybvik H, Steinert M (2020) Investigating the relationship between mental state (workload and affect) and physiology in a control room setting (ship bridge simulator). Cogn Technol Work 22 (1): 95–108
Xu G, Wang S, Zhao D (2021) Transition to sustainable transport: understanding the antecedents of consumer’s intention to adopt electric vehicles from the emotional research perspective. Environ Sci Pollut Res 28 (16): 20362–20374
Yang E, Dorneich M (2015) The effect of time delay on emotion, arousal, and satisfaction in human-robot interaction. In Proceedings of the human factors and ergonomics society annual meeting (Vol. 59, No. 1, pp. 443–447). Los Angeles: SAGE Publications.
Yu Z, Feng Z, Jiang K, Huang Z, Yang Z (2020) Riding personal mobility vehicles on the road: an analysis of the intentions of Chinese users. Cogn Technol Work 22 (4): 801–814
Zeeb K, Härtel M, Buchner A, Schrauf M (2017) Why is steering not the same as braking? The impact of non-driving related tasks on lateral and longitudinal driver interventions during conditionally automated driving. Transport Res Traffic Psychol Behav 50: 65–79
Zhang T, Tao D, Qu X, Zhang X, Lin R, Zhang W (2019) The roles of initial trust and perceived risk in public’s acceptance of automated vehicles. Transp Res Part C Emerg Technol 98: 207–220
Zhang T, Zeng W, Zhang Y, Tao D, Li G, Qu X (2021) What drives people to use automated vehicles? A meta-analytic review. Accid Anal Prev 159: 106270
Zillmann D (1991) Television viewing and physiological arousal. Responding to the screen: Reception and reaction processes. 103–133
Zohar D, Tzischinski O, Epstein R (2003) Effects of energy availability on immediate and delayed emotional reactions to work events. J Appl Psychol 88 (6): 1082
Zurawik MA, Snape R, Carson J (2019) ‘Relaxing way to spend a day’or ‘Best way to keep fit and healthy’. Comparison of leisure experiences in rambling and Nordic walking and their contributions to well-being. Int J Sociol Leisure 2 (4): 347–363
Funding
This study was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant Nos. 51905142, 52172344 and 71971073) and the China Scholarship Council (Grant No. CSC201906695035).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethical approval
All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Informed consent
Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Yu, Z., Jiang, K., Huang, Z. et al. Will I start an automated driving system? Report on the emotions, cognition, and intention of drivers who experienced real-world conditional automated driving. Cogn Tech Work 24, 641–666 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10111-022-00706-2
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10111-022-00706-2