Abstract
Eye tracking gives communication scholars the opportunity to move beyond self-reported measures by examining more precisely how much visual attention is paid to information. However, we lack insight into how eye-tracking data is used in communication research. This literature review provides an overview of how eye tracking is used in communication research by examining published articles from the top-25 ranked communication journals between 2005 and 2015. Our results showed that most eye-tracking research was employed in the field of advertising. Furthermore, most studies used eye tracking to measure (visual) attention and used this as the study’s dependent variable. A wide variety of eye-tracking measures were reported, including fixation time, fixation count, and visual shifts, and a wide variety of eye-tracking devices were used. Our results highlight opportunities for using eye tracking as well as identify other ways of using eye tracking to maximize its potential in communication research.
You have full access to this open access chapter, Download conference paper PDF
Similar content being viewed by others
Keywords
1 Introduction
Eye-movement data gives communication scholars the opportunity to examine more precisely how much visual attention has been paid to information [1–3]. Moreover, eye tracking moves beyond self-reported data by offering a rather objective measure that shows how visual and textual information is processed [4]. Tracking individuals’ eye movements allows researchers to see what individuals find interesting and, possibly, gain insight into how information is perceived [5].
Although eye-tracking technology has been available for more than a century [4], the use of eye-movement data within the field of communication is remarkably limited. This is incredibly surprising as such data is of crucial importance for communication scholars. Mediated communication always involves some kind of material, both visual and textual. Yet, communication researchers have less knowledge about the actual attention people give to this information, as it is often assumed that self-reported measures are an indication of attention. Using eye-movement data, communication scholars may have a better understanding about which information receives attention. For example, eye-movement data can help gain insight into how online health information is processed (e.g., textual or visual information or both [6]). Moreover, eye tracking can be used to make ads more effective or give insight into how people select and read news about public affairs.
This study aims to contribute to this interesting – but understudied – area of research by exploring how eye tracking is currently used within communication research. This study takes the form of a literature review and examines thoroughly and systematically which measures and eye-tracking systems are deployed, and in which context and country the data were collected. We also describe the materials and samples used. As a result of this close examination of previous work, this study offers directions for future communication research in various fields that wish to use eye-movement data.
2 Method
2.1 Search Procedure
To find eligible studies that used eye tracking in communication research, we focused on articles published in the top-25 ranked communication journals from January 1, 2005 to December 8, 2015 (see Appendix for a list of the top-25 ranked communication journals). This top-25 list was based on the 5-year impact factor as indicated by the ISI Web of Knowledge [7] on November 25, 2015. Our focus on the top journals enabled us to provide an overview of the usage of eye tracking in the most important outlets in the field of communication. We used the Communication & Mass Media Complete database to search for eligible studies in the specific journals. Search terms included “eye track*” (All text field) and the name of the journal (Source field): this procedure was repeated for each of the 25 journals.
2.2 Data Extraction
For each article, the following characteristics were extracted: eye-tracking measures, type of eye-tracking measure (i.e., dependent variable, independent variable, mediator, moderator), intended communication measure, study sample (i.e., sample size, type of participants), material format, eye tracker specifications, field, and country (see Table 1).
3 Results
3.1 Study Selection
The search procedure yielded 90 articles. After screening the full-text content of these articles, 70 references were excluded, because they did not report on eye-tracking data (e.g., only mentioned eye tracking as recommended future methodological research approach, reported on previous eye-tracking studies). The remaining 20 articles were screened for data extraction of which the results are reported below.
3.2 Study Characteristics
The included studies mostly reported on eye-tracking data in the field of advertising [8–16], but some eye-tracking studies were also conducted in (public) health [1, 17–19], language studies [20, 21], and computer-mediated communication [22, 23]. Nine studies reported on fixation time [1, 8, 10, 15, 18, 20, 21, 23, 24] or relative fixation time (% fixation time) as eye-tracking measure [9, 14, 16, 20]. Six articles reported on fixation count [10, 18, 20, 21, 23, 24], two on number of fixations-per-second [11, 12], and two on scan paths [23, 24] and visual shifts [24]. Furthermore, one study reported on (re)visit counts [10], one on gaze duration [19], one on dwell time [17], and one on number of jumps between page and ad [13].
The majority of studies used these eye-tracking measures to capture (visual) attention (n = 15) and used these measures as the study’s dependent variable (n = 18). Most material formats included ads, either in the form of print [10, 11, 14], video [1, 9, 11, 12, 15, 16, 20, 24–26], or online [13, 15, 16, 25]. Other non-advertisement material formats included video clips [20], survey questions [21], nutrition labels [18], webpages [23], and products [19]. Although nine articles employed eye tracking on more than 100 participants [8, 9, 13–16, 21, 22, 26], more articles reported on samples below 100 participants (n = 11). The 20 articles showed a wide variety of eye-tracking apparatus used. Seven studies assessed the eye-tracking measures using a Tobii eye tracker [10, 14, 15, 18, 19, 21, 24], three used the ASL Model 504 [1, 20, 23], and two used an SMI [8, 13]. All studies were conducted in Western countries (e.g., the US, Western Europe).
In addition, we noted that different eye trackers measure data with different Hz, which indicates the number of times data is acquired within one second, and different studies use different thresholds with regard to the minimum milliseconds required for a fixation to be registered. Our results showed that some studies use eye trackers with a gaze sample rate of only 25 Hz [22], whereas others collect their data at 120 Hz [8]. Fixations were mostly registered after 100 ms [14, 19, 21, 24], and range from 40 ms [20] to 200 ms [13]. However, most studies did not provide specifications on Hz and ms.
4 Discussion
This literature review explored how eye tracking is used in communication research. The results show that, although a significant number of articles mention the method, only few actually report on eye-tracking data. Eye tracking appears to be mostly used in the field of advertising, and typically quantitatively. In general, it is used to measure (visual) attention to specific content, assessed by a variety of eye-tracking measures, such as fixation time, fixation count, and visual shifts. Furthermore, eye tracking in communication research is employed on various material formats, such as print, video, and webpages.
Our findings suggest that other possible uses of eye tracking, such as usability research or even qualitative usage, seems to be neglected in communication research. Based the low numbers of studies that have employed eye tracking, and because the method could be used for many different goals, we believe that eye tracking has much more potential in communication research. Such potential may include using eye-tracking measures to indicate different communication measures, such as interest [27], applying eye-tracking methodology to other fields of communication research, such as political communication, and extending eye-tracking practices to mobile devices, such as smartphones and tablets [28].
The reported eye-tracking studies often involve rather large samples of more than 100 participants. This is in contrast to non-academic, practical usability research that includes eye tracking, that often uses smaller samples. Although there is no magic number for proper sample size, there is much debate about what sample size is sufficient for eye-tracking research [29]. A meta-analysis may be a potential next step that could reveal interesting insights about effect sizes and power in these studies. Nevertheless, eye tracking can provide various valuable measures, which can give clear insights into the effectiveness of communication materials. However, most of the studies only report one or two measures.
Given the wide variety of eye-tracking devices used in the different studies, it is uncertain whether the eye-tracking data is comparable. Different eye trackers measure data with different Hz, which means that the data acquired from these different devices vary in accuracy. Related to this issue is the difference between studies in the minimum milliseconds required for a fixation to be registered. Whereas some studies use a threshold of 80 ms [8], others register fixations after 100 ms [14] or 200 ms [13]. Moreover, several studies do not specify the minimum milliseconds for a fixation to be registered. It is thus debatable whether we can compare often-used measures such as fixation time and fixation count. We believe that communication research could benefit from more consistency in usage of eye trackers and thresholds and that future studies should make it a priority to include these specifications in their publications.
Another remarkable observation was that all studies were conducted in Western countries (e.g., the US, Australia, The Netherlands, Belgium, and the UK). Future research should be administered in non-Westerns countries, and may even consider comparing attention to communication in Western to non-Western countries. This is especially important because people from different cultures, with different languages and writing systems, may process written messages and even videos differently.
Altogether, this review provides insight into the usage of eye tracking in communication research. Based on our findings, we have formed several directions for further research. Despite these benefits, it is important to note an important limitation – communication research may not be always only published in top-25 ranked communication journals. Thus our review is not a complete overview of all eye-tracking studies. However, it provides insights about eye-tracking usage in the most important outlets in the field of communication.
References
Sanders-Jackson, A.N., Cappella, J.N., Linebarger, D.L., Piotrowski, J.T., O’Keeffe, M., Strasser, A.A.: Visual attention to antismoking PSAs: smoking cues versus other attention-grabbing features. Hum. Commun. Res. 37, 275–292 (2011). doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2010.01402.x
Romano Bergstrom, J.C., Erdman, C., Lakhe, S.: Navigation buttons in web-based surveys respondents’ preferences revisited in the laboratory. Surv. Prac. 9, 1 (2016)
Romano Bergstrom, J.C., Olmsted-Hawala, E.L., Bergstrom, H.C.: Older adults fail to see the periphery during website navigation. Univ. Acc. Inform. Soc. 1–10 (2014). doi:10.1007/s10209-014-0382-z
Poole, A., Ball, L.J.: Eye tracking in human-computer interaction and usability research: current status and future prospects. In: Ghaoui, C. (ed.) Encyclopedia of Human Computer Interaction, pp. 211–219. Idea Group Inc., Pennsylvania (2006)
Duchowski, A.: Eye Tracking Methodology: Theory and Practice. Springer Science & Business Media, London (2007)
Bol, N., Van Weert, J.C.M., Loos, E.F., Romano Bergstrom, J.C., Bolle, S., Smets, E.M.A.: How are online health messages processed? Using eye tracking to predict recall of information in younger and older adults. J. Health Commun 21, 387–396 (2016)
ISI Web of Knowledge: Journal citation reports: journals from subject categories communication (2015). http://admin-apps.webofknowledge.com/JCR/JCR
Boerman, S.C., Van Reijmersdal, E.A., Neijens, P.C.: Using eye tracking to understand the effects of brand placement disclosure types in television programs. J. Advert. 44, 196–207 (2015). 10.1080/00913367.2014.967423
Dix, S., Bellman, S., Haddad, H., Varan, D.: Using interactive program-loyalty banners to reduce TV ad avoidance. J. Advert. Res. 50, 154–160 (2010). doi:10.2501/S0021849910091312
Hartmann, P., Apaolaza, V., Alija, P.: Nature imagery in advertising: attention restoration and memory effects. Int. J. Advert. 32, 183–210 (2013). doi:10.2501/IJA-32-2-183-210
Heath, R.G.: Emotional engagement: how television builds big brands at low attention. J. Advert. Res. 49, 62–73 (2009). doi:10.2501/S0021849909090060
Heath, R.G., Nairn, A.C., Bottomley, P.A.: How effective is creativity? emotive content in TV advertising does not increase attention. J. Advert. Res. 49, 450–463 (2009). doi:10.2501/S0021849909091077
Janssens, W., De Pelsmacker, P., Geuens, M.: Online advertising and congruency effects: it depends on how you look at it. Int. J. Advert. 31, 579–604 (2012). doi:10.2501/IJA-31-3-579-604
Smit, E., Boerman, S., Van Meurs, L.: The power of direct context as revealed by eye tracking: a model tracks relative attention to competing editorial and promotional content. J. Advert. Res. 55, 216–227 (2015). doi:10.2501/JAR-55-2-216-227
Steele, A., Jacobs, D., Siefert, C., Rule, R., Levine, B.: Leveraging synergy and emotion in a multi-platform world: a neuroscience-informed model of engagement. J. Advert. Res. 53, 417–430 (2013). doi:10.2501/JAR-53-4-417-430
Treutler, T., Levine, B., Marci, C.D.: Biometrics and multi-platform messaging: the medium matters. J. Advert. Res. 50, 243–249 (2010). doi:10.2501/S0021849910091415
Bassett-Gunter, R.L., Latimer-Cheung, A.E.: Martin Ginis, K.A., Castelhano, M.: I spy with my little eye: cognitive processing of framed physical activity messages. J. Health Commun. 19, 676–691 (2014). doi:10.1080/10810730.2013.837553
Mackert, M., Champlin, S.E., Pasch, K.E., Weiss, B.D.: Understanding health literacy measurement through eye tracking. J. Health. Commun. 18, 185–196 (2013). doi:10.1080/10810730.2013.825666
Turner, M.M., Skubisz, C., Pandya, S.P., Silverman, M., Austin, L.L.: Predicting visual attention to nutrition information on food products: the influence of motivation and ability. J. Health Commun. 19, 1017–1029 (2014). doi:10.1080/10810730.2013.864726
Beattie, G., Webster, K., Ross, J.: The fixation and processing of the iconic gestures that accompany talk. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 29, 194–213 (2010). doi:10.1177/0261927X09359589
Galesic, M., Tourangeau, R., Couper, M.P., Conrad, F.G.: Eye-tracking data: new insights on response order effects and other cognitive shortcuts in survey responding. Public. Opin. Q. 72, 892–913 (2008). doi:10.1093/poq/nfn059
Bente, G., Rüggenberg, S., Krämer, N.C., Eschenburg, F.: Avatar-mediated networking: increasing social presence and interpersonal trust in net-based collaborations. Hum. Commun. Res. 34, 287–318 (2008). doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2008.00322.x
Pan, B., Hembrooke, H., Joachims, T., Lorigo, L., Gay, G., Granka, L.: In Google we trust: users’ decisions on rank, position, and relevance. J. Comput. Mediat. Commun. 12, 801–823 (2007). doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2007.00351.x
Perego, E., Del Missier, F., Porta, M., Mosconi, M.: The cognitive effectiveness of subtitle processing. Media. Psychol. 13, 243–272 (2010). doi:10.1080/15213269.2010.502873
Neale, L., Bellman, S., Treleaven-Hassard, S., Robinson, J., Varan, D.: Unlocking the ‘reminder’ potential when viewers pause programs: results from a laboratory test of a new online medium. J. Advert. Res. 53, 444–454 (2013). doi:10.2501/JAR-53-4-444-454
Siefert, C., Gallent, J., Jacobs, D., Levine, B., Stipp, H., Marci, C.: Biometric and eye-tracking insights into the efficiency of information processing of television advertising during fast-forward viewing. Int. J. Advert. 27, 425–446 (2008). doi:10.2501/S0265048708080050
Olmsted-Hawala, E.L., Holland, T., Quach, V.: Usability testing. In: Romano Bergstrom, J., Schall, A.J. (eds.) Eye Tracking in User Experience Design, pp. 49–80. Elsevier, Amsterdam (2014)
He, J., Siu, C., Chaparro, B., Strohl, J.: Mobile. In: Romano Bergstrom, J., Schall, A.J. (eds.) Eye Tracking in User Experience Design, pp. 255–290. Elsevier, Amsterdam (2014)
Loos, E.F., Romano Bergstrom, J.: Older adults. In: Romano Bergstrom, J., Schall, A.J. (eds.) Eye Tracking in User Experience Design, pp. 313–329. Elsevier, Amsterdam (2014)
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Editor information
Editors and Affiliations
Appendix: Top-25 Ranked Communication Journals (Alphabetical Order)
Appendix: Top-25 Ranked Communication Journals (Alphabetical Order)
Communication Monographs
Communication Research
Communication Theory
Health Communication
Human Communication Research
Information Communication & Society
International Journal of Advertising
International Journal of Press-Politics
Journal of Advertising
Journal of Advertising Research
Journal of Communication
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication
Journal of Health Communication
Journal of Language and Social Psychology
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships
Management Communication Quarterly
Media Psychology
New Media & Society
Personal Relationships
Political Communication
Public Opinion Quarterly
Public Understanding of Science
Research on Language and Social Interaction
Science Communication
Telecommunications Policy
Rights and permissions
Copyright information
© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland
About this paper
Cite this paper
Bol, N., Boerman, S.C., Romano Bergstrom, J.C., Kruikemeier, S. (2016). An Overview of How Eye Tracking Is Used in Communication Research. In: Antona, M., Stephanidis, C. (eds) Universal Access in Human-Computer Interaction. Methods, Techniques, and Best Practices. UAHCI 2016. Lecture Notes in Computer Science(), vol 9737. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-40250-5_40
Download citation
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-40250-5_40
Published:
Publisher Name: Springer, Cham
Print ISBN: 978-3-319-40249-9
Online ISBN: 978-3-319-40250-5
eBook Packages: Computer ScienceComputer Science (R0)