[go: up one dir, main page]
More Web Proxy on the site http://driver.im/Jump to content

N100

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
(Redirected from N100 (neuroscience))

In neuroscience, the N100 or N1 is a large, negative-going evoked potential measured by electroencephalography (its equivalent in magnetoencephalography is the M100); it peaks in adults between 80 and 120 milliseconds after the onset of a stimulus, and is distributed mostly over the fronto-central region of the scalp. It is elicited by any unpredictable stimulus in the absence of task demands. It is often referred to with the following P200 evoked potential as the "N100-P200" or "N1-P2" complex. While most research focuses on auditory stimuli, the N100 also occurs for visual (see visual N1, including an illustration),[1] olfactory,[2] heat,[3] pain,[3] balance,[4] respiration blocking,[5] and somatosensory stimuli.[6]

The auditory N100 is generated by a network of neural populations in the primary and association auditory cortices in the superior temporal gyrus in Heschl's gyrus[7] and planum temporale.[8] It also could be generated in the frontal and motor areas.[9] The area generating it is larger in the right hemisphere than the left.[7]

The N100 is preattentive and involved in perception because its amplitude is strongly dependent upon such things as the rise time of the onset of a sound,[10] its loudness,[11] interstimulus interval with other sounds,[12] and the comparative frequency of a sound as its amplitude increases in proportion to how much a sound differs in frequency from a preceding one.[13] Neuromagnetic research has linked it further to perception by finding that the auditory cortex has a tonotopic organization to N100.[14] However, it also shows a link to a person's arousal[15] and selective attention.[16] N100 is decreased when a person controls the creation of auditory stimuli,[17] such as their own voice.[18]

Types

[edit]

There are three subtypes of adult auditory N100.[9]

  • N100b or vertex N100, peaking at 100 ms.
  • T-complex N100a, largest at temporal electrodes at 75 ms
  • T-complex N100c, follows N100a and peaks at about 130 ms. The two T-complex N100 evoked potentials are created by auditory association cortices in the superior temporal gyri.

Elicitation

[edit]

The N100 is often known as the "auditory N100" because it is elicited by perception of auditory stimuli. Specifically, it has been found to be sensitive to things such as the predictability of an auditory stimulus, and special features of speech sounds such as voice onset time.

During sleep

[edit]

It occurs during both REM and NREM stages of sleep though its time is slightly delayed.[19] During stage 2 NREM it seems responsible for the production of K-complexes.[20] N100 is reduced following total sleep deprivation and this associates with an impaired ability to consolidate memories.[21]

Stimulus repetition

[edit]

The N100 depends upon unpredictability of stimulus: it is weaker when stimuli are repetitive, and stronger when they are random. When subjects are allowed to control stimuli, using a switch, the N100 may decrease.[17] This effect has been linked to intelligence, as the N100 attenuation for self-controlled stimuli occurs the most strongly (i.e., the N100 shrinks the most) in individuals who are also evaluated as having high intelligence. Indeed, researchers have found that in those with Down syndrome "the amplitude of the self-evoked response actually exceeded that of the machine-evoked potential".[17] Being warned about an upcoming stimulus also reduces its N100.[22]

The amplitude of N100 shows refractoriness upon repetition of a stimulus; in other words, it decreases at first upon repeated presentations of the stimulus, but after a short period of silence it returns to its previous level.[9] Paradoxically, at short repetition the second N100 is enhanced both for sound[23] and somatosensory stimuli.[6]

With paired clicks, the second N100 is reduced due to sensory gating.[24]

Voice onset time

[edit]

The difference between many consonants is their voice onset time (VOT), the interval between consonant release (onset) and the start of rhythmic vocal cord vibrations in the vowel. The voiced stop consonants /b/, /d/ and /g/ have a short VOT, and unvoiced stop consonants /p/, /t/ and /k/ long VOTs. The N100 plays a role in recognizing the difference and categorizing these sounds: speech stimuli with a short 0 to +30 ms voice onset time evoke a single N100 response but those with a longer (+30 ms and longer) evoked two N100 peaks and these are linked to the consonant release and vocal cord vibration onset.[25][26]

Top-down influences

[edit]

Traditionally, 50 to 150 ms evoked potentials were considered too short to be influenced by top-down influences from the prefrontal cortex. However, it is now known that sensory input is processed by the occipital cortex by 56 ms and this is communicated to the dorsolateral frontal cortex where it arrives by 80 ms.[27] Research also finds that the modulation effects upon N100 are affected by prefrontal cortex lesions.[28] These higher-level areas create the attentive, repetition, and arousal modulations upon the sensory area processing reflected in N100.[29]

Another top-down influence upon N100 has been suggested to be efference copies from a person's intended movements so that the stimulation that results from them are not processed.[30] A person's own voice produces a reduced N100[18] as does the effect of a self-initiated compared to externally created perturbation upon balance.[31]

Development in children

[edit]

The N100 is a slow-developing evoked potential. From one to four years of age, a positive evoked potential, P100, is the predominant peak.[32] Older children start to develop a negative evoked potential at 200 ms that dominates evoked potentials until adolescence;[33] this potential is identical to the adult N100 in scalp topography and elicitation, but with a much later onset. The magnetic M100 (measured by MEG rather than EEG) is, likewise, less robust in children than in adults.[34] An adult-like N100-P200 complex only develops after 10 years of age.[35]

The various types of N100 mature at different times. Their maturation also varies with the side of the brain: N100a in the left hemisphere is mature before three years of age but this does not happen in the right hemisphere until seven or eight years of age.[33]

Clinical use

[edit]

The N100 may be used to test for abnormalities in the auditory system where verbal or behavioral responses cannot be used,[36] such with individuals in coma; in such cases, it can help predict the probability of recovery.[37][38] Another application is in assessing the optimal level of sedation in intensive critical care.[39]

High density mapping of the location of the generators of M100 is being researched as a means of presurgical neuromapping needed for neurosurgery.[40]

Many cognitive or other mental impairments are associated with changes in the N100 response, including the following:

  • There is some evidence that the N100 is affected in those with dyslexia and specific language impairment.[41]
  • The sensory gating effect upon N100 with paired clicks is reduced in those with schizophrenia.[24]
  • In individuals with tinnitus, those with smaller N100 are less distressed than those with larger amplitudes.[42]
  • Migraine is associated with an increase rather than decrease in N100 amplitude with repetition of the high-intensity stimulation.[43]
  • Headache sufferers also have more reactive N100 to somatosensory input than nonsufferers[44]

The N100 is 10 to 20% larger than normal when the auditory stimulus is synchronized with the diastolic phase of the cardiac blood pressure pulse.[45]

Relationship to mismatch negativity

[edit]

The Mismatch negativity (MMN) is an evoked potential that occurs at roughly the same time as N100 in response to rare auditory events. It differs from the N100 in that:

  • They are generated in different locations.[46]
  • The MMN occurs too late to be an N100.[47]
  • The MMN, unlike N100, may be elicited by stimulus omissions (i.e., not hearing a stimulus when you expect to hear one).[48]

Though this suggests that they are separate processes, arguments have been made that this is not necessarily so and that they are created by the "relative activation of multiple cortical areas contributing to both of these 'components'".[49]

History

[edit]

Pauline A. Davis at Harvard University first recorded the wave peak now identified with N100.[50] The present use of the N1 to describe this peak originates in 1966[51] and N100 later in the mid 1970s.[52] The origin of the wave for a long time was unknown and only linked to the auditory cortex in 1970.[9][53]

Due to magnetoencephalography, research is increasingly done upon M100, the magnetic counterpart of the electroencephalographic N100. Unlike electrical fields which face the high resistance of the skull and generate secondary or volume currents, magnetic fields which are orthogonal to them have a homogeneous permeability through the skull. This enables the location of sources generating fields that are tangent to the head surface with an accuracy of a few millimeters.[54] New techniques, such as event-related beam-forming with magnetoencephalography, allow sufficiently accurate location of M100 sources to be clinically useful for preparing surgery upon the brain.[40]

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ Warnke, A.; Remschmidt, H.; Hennighausen, K. (1994). "Verbal information processing in dyslexia--data from a follow-up experiment of neuro-psychological aspects and EEG". Acta Paedopsychiatrica. 56 (3): 203–208. PMID 7521558.
  2. ^ Pause, B. M.; Sojka, B.; Krauel, K.; Ferstl, R. (1996). "The nature of the late positive complex within the olfactory event-related potential (OERP)". Psychophysiology. 33 (4): 376–384. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb01062.x. PMID 8753937.
  3. ^ a b Greffrath, W.; Baumgärtner, U.; Treede, R. D. (2007). "Peripheral and central components of habituation of heat pain perception and evoked potentials in humans". Pain. 132 (3): 301–311. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2007.04.026. PMID 17533117. S2CID 29266108.
  4. ^ Quant, S.; Maki, B. E.; McIlroy, W. E. (2005). "The association between later cortical potentials and later phases of postural reactions evoked by perturbations to upright stance". Neuroscience Letters. 381 (3): 269–274. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2005.02.015. PMID 15896482. S2CID 24770418.
  5. ^ Chan, P. -Y. S.; Davenport, P. W. (2008). "Respiratory-related evoked potential measures of respiratory sensory gating". Journal of Applied Physiology. 105 (4): 1106–1113. doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.90722.2008. PMC 4347743. PMID 18719232.
  6. ^ a b Wang, A. L.; Mouraux, A.; Liang, M.; Iannetti, G. D. (2008). Lauwereyns, Jan (ed.). "The Enhancement of the N1 Wave Elicited by Sensory Stimuli Presented at Very Short Inter-Stimulus Intervals is a General Feature across Sensory Systems". PLOS ONE. 3 (12): e3929. Bibcode:2008PLoSO...3.3929W. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0003929. PMC 2597742. PMID 19081790.
  7. ^ a b Zouridakis, G.; Simos, P. G.; Papanicolaou, A. C. (1998). "Multiple bilaterally asymmetric cortical sources account for the auditory N1m component". Brain Topography. 10 (3): 183–189. doi:10.1023/A:1022246825461. PMID 9562539. S2CID 1743975.
  8. ^ Godey, B.; Schwartz, D.; De Graaf, J. B.; Chauvel, P.; Liégeois-Chauvel, C. (2001). "Neuromagnetic source localization of auditory evoked fields and intracerebral evoked potentials: A comparison of data in the same patients" (PDF). Clinical Neurophysiology. 112 (10): 1850–1859. doi:10.1016/s1388-2457(01)00636-8. PMID 11595143. S2CID 6501656.
  9. ^ a b c d Näätänen, R.; Picton, T. (1987). "The N1 wave of the human electric and magnetic response to sound: A review and an analysis of the component structure". Psychophysiology. 24 (4): 375–425. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1987.tb00311.x. PMID 3615753.
  10. ^ Spreng, M. (1980). "Influence of impulsive and fluctuating noise upon physiological excitations and short-time readaptation". Scandinavian Audiology. Supplementum (Suppl 12): 299–306. PMID 6939101.
  11. ^ Keidel, W. D.; Spreng, M. (1965). "Neurophysiological Evidence for the Stevens Power Function in Man". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 38 (2): 191–195. Bibcode:1965ASAJ...38..191K. doi:10.1121/1.1909629. PMID 14341718.
  12. ^ Davis, H.; Mast, T.; Yoshie, N.; Zerlin, S. (1966). "The slow response of the human cortex to auditory stimuli: Recovery process". Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 21 (2): 105–113. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(66)90118-0. PMID 4162003.
  13. ^ Butler, R. A. (1968). "Effect of changes in stimulus frequency and intensity on habituation of the human vertex potential". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 44 (4): 945–950. Bibcode:1968ASAJ...44..945B. doi:10.1121/1.1911233. PMID 5683660.
  14. ^ Pantev, C.; Hoke, M.; Lehnertz, K.; Lütkenhöner, B.; Anogianakis, G.; Wittkowski, W. (1988). "Tonotopic organization of the human auditory cortex revealed by transient auditory evoked magnetic fields". Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 69 (2): 160–170. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(88)90211-8. PMID 2446835.
  15. ^ Nash, A. J.; Williams, C. S. (1982). "Effects of preparatory set and task demands on auditory event-related potentials". Biological Psychology. 15 (1–2): 15–31. doi:10.1016/0301-0511(82)90028-x. PMID 7138998. S2CID 43038773.
  16. ^ Hillyard, S. A.; Hink, R. F.; Schwent, V. L.; Picton, T. W. (1973). "Electrical signs of selective attention in the human brain". Science. 182 (4108): 177–180. Bibcode:1973Sci...182..177H. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.465.3727. doi:10.1126/science.182.4108.177. PMID 4730062. S2CID 14430569.
  17. ^ a b c Schafer, E. W.; Marcus, M. M. (1973). "Self-stimulation alters human sensory brain responses". Science. 181 (4095): 175–177. Bibcode:1973Sci...181..175S. doi:10.1126/science.181.4095.175. PMID 4711735. S2CID 145362025.
  18. ^ a b Curio, G.; Neuloh, G.; Numminen, J.; Jousmäki, V.; Hari, R. (2000). "Speaking modifies voice-evoked activity in the human auditory cortex". Human Brain Mapping. 9 (4): 183–191. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(200004)9:4<183::AID-HBM1>3.0.CO;2-Z. PMC 6871984. PMID 10770228.
  19. ^ Nordby, H.; Hugdahl, K.; Stickgold, R.; Bronnick, K. S.; Hobson, J. A. (1996). "Event-related potentials (ERPs) to deviant auditory stimuli during sleep and waking". NeuroReport. 7 (5): 1082–1086. doi:10.1097/00001756-199604100-00026. PMID 8804056. S2CID 40303719.
  20. ^ Niiyama, Y.; Satoh, N.; Kutsuzawa, O.; Hishikawa, Y. (1996). "Electrophysiological evidence suggesting that sensory stimuli of unknown origin induce spontaneous K-complexes". Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 98 (5): 394–400. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(96)95103-2. PMID 8647042.
  21. ^ Mograss, M. A.; Guillem, F.; Brazzini-Poisson, V.; Godbout, R. (2009). "The effects of total sleep deprivation on recognition memory processes: A study of event-related potential". Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 91 (4): 343–352. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2009.01.008. PMID 19340944. S2CID 35985772.
  22. ^ Schafer, E. W.; Amochaev, A.; Russell, M. J. (1981). "Knowledge of stimulus timing attenuates human evoked cortical potentials". Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 52 (1): 9–17. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(81)90183-8. PMID 6166459.
  23. ^ Budd, T. W.; Michie, P. T. (1994). "Facilitation of the N1 peak of the auditory ERP at short stimulus intervals". NeuroReport. 5 (18): 2513–2516. doi:10.1097/00001756-199412000-00027. PMID 7696592.
  24. ^ a b Hanlon, F. M.; Miller, G. A.; Thoma, R. J.; Irwin, J.; Jones, A.; Moses, S. N.; Huang, M.; Weisend, M. P.; Paulson, K. M.; Edgar, J. C.; Adler, L. E.; Cañive, J. M. (2005). "Distinct M50 and M100 auditory gating deficits in schizophrenia". Psychophysiology. 42 (4): 417–427. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00299.x. PMID 16008770.
  25. ^ Steinschneider, M.; Volkov, I. O.; Fishman, Y. I.; Oya, H.; Arezzo, J. C.; Howard III, M. A. (2004). "Intracortical Responses in Human and Monkey Primary Auditory Cortex Support a Temporal Processing Mechanism for Encoding of the Voice Onset Time Phonetic Parameter". Cerebral Cortex. 15 (2): 170–186. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhh120. PMID 15238437.
  26. ^ Steinschneider, M.; Volkov, I. O.; Noh, M. D.; Garell, P. C.; Howard III, M. A. (1999). "Temporal encoding of the voice onset time phonetic parameter by field potentials recorded directly from human auditory cortex". Journal of Neurophysiology. 82 (5): 2346–2357. doi:10.1152/jn.1999.82.5.2346. PMID 10561410.
  27. ^ Foxe, J.; Simpson, G. (2002). "Flow of activation from V1 to frontal cortex in humans". Experimental Brain Research. 142 (1): 139–150. doi:10.1007/s00221-001-0906-7. PMID 11797091. S2CID 25506401.
  28. ^ Blenner, J. L.; Yingling, C. D. (1994). "Effects of prefrontal cortex lesions on visual evoked potential augmenting/reducing". The International Journal of Neuroscience. 78 (3–4): 145–156. doi:10.3109/00207459408986053. PMID 7883451.
  29. ^ Coull, J. T. (1998). "Neural correlates of attention and arousal: Insights from electrophysiology, functional neuroimaging and psychopharmacology". Progress in Neurobiology. 55 (4): 343–361. doi:10.1016/S0301-0082(98)00011-2. PMID 9654384. S2CID 26194262. PDF
  30. ^ Kudo, N.; Nakagome, K.; Kasai, K.; Araki, T.; Fukuda, M.; Kato, N.; Iwanami, A. (2004). "Effects of corollary discharge on event-related potentials during selective attention task in healthy men and women". Neuroscience Research. 48 (1): 59–64. doi:10.1016/j.neures.2003.09.008. PMID 14687881. S2CID 37957344.
  31. ^ Mochizuki, G.; Sibley, K. M.; Cheung, H. J.; McIlroy, W. E. (2009). "Cortical activity prior to predictable postural instability: Is there a difference between self-initiated and externally-initiated perturbations?". Brain Research. 1279: 29–36. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2009.04.050. PMID 19422812. S2CID 12170405.
  32. ^ Kushnerenko, E.; Ceponiene, R.; Balan, P.; Fellman, V.; Huotilaine, M.; Näätäne, R. (2002). "Maturation of the auditory event-related potentials during the first year of life". NeuroReport. 13 (1): 47–51. doi:10.1097/00001756-200201210-00014. PMID 11924892. S2CID 33934033.
  33. ^ a b Pang, E. W.; Taylor, M. J. (2000). "Tracking the development of the N1 from age 3 to adulthood: An examination of speech and non-speech stimuli". Clinical Neurophysiology. 111 (3): 388–397. doi:10.1016/S1388-2457(99)00259-X. PMID 10699397. S2CID 37029252.
  34. ^ Paetau, R.; Ahonen, A.; Salonen, O.; Sams, M. (1995). "Auditory evoked magnetic fields to tones and pseudowords in healthy children and adults". Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology. 12 (2): 177–185. doi:10.1097/00004691-199503000-00008. PMID 7797632. S2CID 33059979.
  35. ^ Shibasaki, H.; Miyazaki, M. (1992). "Event-related potential studies in infants and children". Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology. 9 (3): 408–418. doi:10.1097/00004691-199207010-00007. PMID 1517407.
  36. ^ Hyde, M. (1997). "The N1 response and its applications". Audiology and Neuro-Otology. 2 (5): 281–307. doi:10.1159/000259253. PMID 9390837.
  37. ^ Fischer, C.; Morlet, D.; Giard, M. (2000). "Mismatch negativity and N100 in comatose patients". Audiology and Neuro-Otology. 5 (3–4): 192–197. doi:10.1159/000013880. PMID 10859413. S2CID 42959451.
  38. ^ Fischer, C.; Luauté, J.; Adeleine, P.; Morlet, D. (2004). "Predictive value of sensory and cognitive evoked potentials for awakening from coma". Neurology. 63 (4): 669–673. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000134670.10384.e2. PMID 15326240. S2CID 31825723.
  39. ^ Yppärilä, H.; Nunes, S.; Korhonen, I.; Partanen, J.; Ruokonen, E. (2004). "The effect of interruption to propofol sedation on auditory event-related potentials and electroencephalogram in intensive care patients". Critical Care. 8 (6): R483–R490. doi:10.1186/cc2984. PMC 1065074. PMID 15566595.
  40. ^ a b Cheyne, D.; Bostan, A. C.; Gaetz, W.; Pang, E. W. (2007). "Event-related beamforming: A robust method for presurgical functional mapping using MEG". Clinical Neurophysiology. 118 (8): 1691–1704. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2007.05.064. PMID 17587643. S2CID 5900519.
  41. ^ Shaul S. (2007). Evoked response potentials (ERPs) in the study of dyslexia: A review. pp. 51–91. In (Breznitz Z. Editor) Brain Research in Language. Springer ISBN 978-0-387-74979-2
  42. ^ Delb, W.; Strauss, D. J.; Low, Y. F.; Seidler, H.; Rheinschmitt, A.; Wobrock, T.; d’Amelio, R. (2008). "Alterations in Event Related Potentials (ERP) Associated with Tinnitus Distress and Attention". Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback. 33 (4): 211–221. doi:10.1007/s10484-008-9065-y. PMID 18836827.
  43. ^ Wang, W.; Timsit-Berthier, M.; Schoenen, J. (1996). "Intensity dependence of auditory evoked potentials is pronounced in migraine: An indication of cortical potentiation and low serotonergic neurotransmission?". Neurology. 46 (5): 1404–1409. doi:10.1212/wnl.46.5.1404. PMID 8628490. S2CID 2645081.
  44. ^ Marlowe, N. (1995). "Somatosensory evoked potentials and headache: A further examination of the central theory". Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 39 (2): 119–131. doi:10.1016/0022-3999(94)00072-d. PMID 7595870.
  45. ^ Sandman, C. A.; O'Halloran, J. P.; Isenhart, R. (1984). "Is there an evoked vascular response?". Science. 224 (4655): 1355–1357. Bibcode:1984Sci...224.1355S. doi:10.1126/science.6729458. PMID 6729458.
  46. ^ Alho, K. (1995). "Cerebral generators of mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic counterpart (MMNm) elicited by sound changes". Ear and Hearing. 16 (1): 38–51. doi:10.1097/00003446-199502000-00004. PMID 7774768. S2CID 31698419.
  47. ^ Näätänen, R.; Alho, K. (1995). "Mismatch negativity—a unique measure of sensory processing in audition". The International Journal of Neuroscience. 80 (1–4): 317–337. doi:10.3109/00207459508986107. PMID 7775056.
  48. ^ Yabe, H.; Tervaniemi, M.; Sinkkonen, J.; Huotilainen, M.; Ilmoniemi, R. J.; Näätänen, R. (1998). "Temporal window of integration of auditory information in the human brain". Psychophysiology. 35 (5): 615–619. doi:10.1017/s0048577298000183. PMID 9715105.
  49. ^ May, P. J.; Tiitinen, H. (2004). "The MMN is a derivative of the auditory N100 response". Neurology & Clinical Neurophysiology. 2004: 20. PMID 16012601.
  50. ^ Davis PA. (1939). Effects of acoustic stimuli on the waking human brain. J Neurophysiol 2: 494–499 abstract
  51. ^ Davis, H; Zerlin, S (1966). "Acoustic relations of the human vertex potential". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 39 (1): 109–16. Bibcode:1966ASAJ...39..109D. doi:10.1121/1.1909858. PMID 5904525.
  52. ^ Donchin, E.; Tueting, P.; Ritter, W.; Kutas, M.; Heffley, E. (1975). "On the independence of the CNV and the P300 components of the human averaged evoked potential". Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 38 (5): 449–461. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(75)90187-x. PMID 50170.
  53. ^ Vaughan Jr, H. G.; Ritter, W. (1970). "The sources of auditory evoked responses recorded from the human scalp". Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 28 (4): 360–367. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(70)90228-2. PMID 4191187.
  54. ^ Hämäläinen M, Hari R, Ilmoniemi RJ, Knuutila J. (1993). Magnetoencephalography-theory, instrumentation, and applications to noninvasive studies of the working human brain. Reviews of modern Physics. 65: 413–497. OCLC 197237696