[go: up one dir, main page]
More Web Proxy on the site http://driver.im/
You seem to have javascript disabled. Please note that many of the page functionalities won't work as expected without javascript enabled.
 
 
Sign in to use this feature.

Years

Between: -

Subjects

remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline
remove_circle_outline

Journals

Article Types

Countries / Regions

Search Results (25)

Search Parameters:
Keywords = semantic memory disorders

Order results
Result details
Results per page
Select all
Export citation of selected articles as:
12 pages, 262 KiB  
Review
Different Markers of Semantic–Lexical Impairment Allow One to Obtain Different Information on the Conversion from MCI to AD: A Narrative Review of an Ongoing Research Program
by Davide Quaranta, Camillo Marra, Maria Gabriella Vita and Guido Gainotti
Brain Sci. 2024, 14(11), 1128; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14111128 - 8 Nov 2024
Viewed by 656
Abstract
Background: In this narrative review, we have surveyed results obtained from a research program dealing with the role of semantic memory disorders as a predictor of progression from mild cognitive impairment (MCI) to Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Objectives: In this research program, we have [...] Read more.
Background: In this narrative review, we have surveyed results obtained from a research program dealing with the role of semantic memory disorders as a predictor of progression from mild cognitive impairment (MCI) to Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Objectives: In this research program, we have taken into account many different putative markers, provided of a different complexity in the study of the semantic network. These markers ranged from the number of words produced on a semantic fluency task to the following: (a) the discrepancy between scores obtained on semantic vs. phonemic word fluency tests; (b) the presence, at the single-word level, of features (such as a loss of low typical words on a category verbal fluency task) typical of a degraded semantic system; or (c) the presence of more complex phenomena (such as the semantic distance between consecutively produced word pairs) concerning the organization of the semantic network. In the present review, all these studies have been presented, providing separate subsections for (a) methods, (b) results, and (c) a short discussion. Some tentative general conclusions have been drawn at the end of the review. We found that at baseline all these markers are impaired in MCI patients who will later convert to AD, but also that they do not necessarily show a linear worsening during the progression to AD and allow one to make different predictions about the time of development of AD. Our conclusions were that, rather than searching for the best marker of conversion, we should use a range of different markers allowing us to obtain the information most appropriate to the goal of our investigation. Full article
14 pages, 1059 KiB  
Article
Selecting a Brief Cognitive Screening Test Based on Patient Profile: It Is Never Too Early to Start
by Gemma García-Lluch, Ariadna Muedra-Moreno, Mar García-Zamora, Beatriz Gómez, Rafael Sánchez-Roy and Lucrecia Moreno
J. Clin. Med. 2024, 13(19), 6009; https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm13196009 - 9 Oct 2024
Viewed by 860
Abstract
Introduction: Cognitive impairment, marked by a decline in memory and attention, is frequently underdiagnosed, complicating effective management. Cardiovascular risk factors (CVR) and anticholinergic burden (ACB) are significant contributors to dementia risk, with ACB often stemming from medications prescribed for neuropsychiatric disorders. This [...] Read more.
Introduction: Cognitive impairment, marked by a decline in memory and attention, is frequently underdiagnosed, complicating effective management. Cardiovascular risk factors (CVR) and anticholinergic burden (ACB) are significant contributors to dementia risk, with ACB often stemming from medications prescribed for neuropsychiatric disorders. This study evaluates cognitive profiles through three brief cognitive tests, analyzing the impact of CVR and ACB presence. Methods: This cross-sectional study was performed between 2019 and 2023 in community pharmacies and an outpatient clinic in Valencia, Spain. Eligible participants were patients with subjective memory complaints 50 years or older with clinical records of cardiovascular factors. Patients with conflicting information regarding diabetes diagnosis or not taking concomitant medications were excluded. Three brief cognitive tests (Memory Impairment Screening (MIS), Semantic Verbal Fluency Test, and SPMSQ) were assessed. CVR was calculated using the European SCORE2 table, and ACB was assessed using the CALS scale. Results: Among 172 patients with memory complaints and CVR factors, 60% failed at least one cognitive test. These patients were on significantly more medications and had higher blood pressure and HbA1c levels. An increase in CVR and ACB was associated with more failed tests. Additionally, elevated SCORE2 scores were associated with a greater failure rate on the MIS test, while patients with elevated ACB more frequently failed the SPMSQ test. Conclusions: Selecting an adequate brief cognitive test according to patients’ characteristics offers an opportunity to screen patients who are probably cognitively impaired. Whereas the MIS test may be helpful for patients with cardiovascular risk, SPMSQ stands out among patients with significant ACB. Full article
(This article belongs to the Section Mental Health)
Show Figures

Figure 1

Figure 1
<p>Participant selection diagram for this study. Created in BioRender. San Pablo CEU, F. (2024) <a href="http://BioRender.com" target="_blank">BioRender.com</a>. Group 1: High cardiovascular risk (CVR) and anticholinergic burden (ACB) ≥ 3 (red); Group 2: High CVR and ACB &lt; 3 (orange); Group 3: No CVR and ACB ≥ 3 (purple); Group 4: No significant CVR and ACB &lt; 3 (green, reference group). Dark purple indicates high CVR; light purple indicates ACB &lt; 3, according to the CALS scale. Abbreviations: ACB: Anticholinergic Burden; HDL: High-Density Lipoprotein Cholesterol; CVR: Cardiovascular Risk; SBP: Systolic Blood Pressure.</p>
Full article ">Figure 2
<p>Relationship between groups and the number of failed tests. Groups 1 and 3 have more patients failing one test, with a high anticholinergic burden (ACB). Group 1 has fewer patients failing one neuropsychological test than Group 3, but more patients fail all three tests. Groups 2 and 4 have a low ACB, with Group 2 having a high cardiovascular risk (CVR). Group 1: High CVR and ACB &gt; 2 (red); Group 2: High CVR and ACB &lt; 3 (orange); Group 3: No CVR and ACB &gt; 2 (purple); Group 4: No significant CVR and ACB &lt; 3 (green, reference group).</p>
Full article ">
9 pages, 672 KiB  
Article
Increased Semantic Memorization in Children with ADHD during a Paradigm of Motor Priming: Exploratory Findings
by Ana Moscoso, Clarisse Louisin, Simona Caldani, Mickael Worms Ehrminger, Mylene Fefeu, Eric Acquaviva, Richard Delorme and Maria Pia Bucci
Children 2024, 11(7), 787; https://doi.org/10.3390/children11070787 - 28 Jun 2024
Viewed by 861
Abstract
Aim: The aim was to evaluate the effect of body actions on learning process, particularly semantic memory capabilities in drug-naïve children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Method: Thirty children had to listen to a story which was repeated three times in a [...] Read more.
Aim: The aim was to evaluate the effect of body actions on learning process, particularly semantic memory capabilities in drug-naïve children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Method: Thirty children had to listen to a story which was repeated three times in a row and then a fourth time five minutes later. After each listen, the child was asked what she/he remembered from the story. The whole sample was split randomly into three subgroups of equal IQ (mean 102.2 ± 12.7), age (mean age 8 ± 0.6 years), sex (ratio female to male 1:5) and severity of ADHD symptoms (34.2 ± 7.4); a G1 “Freeze” subgroup, which implied listening to the story while sitting on a chair without moving; a G2 “Minimal” subgroup, which implied listening to the story while sitting on a chair but free movement was allowed; a G3 “Prescribed movement” subgroup, which implied listening to the story standing up, while copying the experimenter movements that mimicked the actions told in the story. Results: Although our sample was limited in size, interestingly, children in the G3 subgroup showed the highest short-term semantic memory retention compared to G1. In all subgroups, repetition allowed an increase in performance. Conclusions: Our exploratory findings stress the positive role of movement in children with ADHD to increase semantic memorization. Hyperactivity may counteract the deficit of memorization related to attention impairment in children with ADHD. Our results may encourage parents or teachers to allow children with ADHD to move around during short-term memory-retention tasks. Full article
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Updates on Child Neuropsychiatry)
Show Figures

Figure 1

Figure 1
<p>Semantic memory performance in the three studied groups (G1, G2, G3) at the four timepoints. (<b>a</b>) Timepoint 1, (<b>b</b>) Timepoint 2, (<b>c</b>) Timepoint 3, (<b>d</b>) Timepoint 4.</p>
Full article ">Figure 2
<p>Learning curves of the three studied conditions. Median evolution at the four timepoints.</p>
Full article ">
15 pages, 304 KiB  
Article
Psychotherapy Incorporating Equine Interaction as a Complementary Therapeutic Intervention for Young Adults in a Residential Treatment Program during the COVID-19 Pandemic
by Katie Holtcamp, Molly C. Nicodemus, Tommy Phillips, David Christiansen, Brian J. Rude, Peter L. Ryan and Karen Galarneau
COVID 2023, 3(10), 1571-1585; https://doi.org/10.3390/covid3100107 - 4 Oct 2023
Cited by 5 | Viewed by 2163
Abstract
Substance use disorder has become an epidemic in the young adult population across the United States, and these numbers rose during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychotherapy incorporating equine interaction has emerged to show promise in the mental health community as a complementary form of [...] Read more.
Substance use disorder has become an epidemic in the young adult population across the United States, and these numbers rose during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychotherapy incorporating equine interaction has emerged to show promise in the mental health community as a complementary form of therapy for this age group and offered a viable treatment option during the pandemic due to the outdoor nature of the treatment environment. However, research concerning its use within a residential treatment program was lacking. The objective of this study was to evaluate the efficacy of psychotherapy incorporating equine interaction in a residential treatment program during the COVID-19 pandemic for developing an emotionally safe environment for learning for young adults. Participants (ages 18–25 years) were those in a substance abuse residential treatment program utilizing psychotherapy incorporating equine interaction during the COVID-19 pandemic. Participants were involved in weekly equine therapy for 2–7 weeks. Participants were divided according to length of stay at the residential facility and participation level with equine interactive activities. Assessment of emotional safety and long-term memory development was performed at the beginning and end of the treatment program. The development of memories centered around equine information that was covered during the treatment program. Semantic memory was assessed using a self-reporting knowledge exam and procedural memory was assessed using a skill evaluation. Emotional safety was determined using a self-reporting survey instrument. Paired t-tests determined significant improvement in emotional safety (p = 0.02) and semantic (p = 0.01) and procedural (p = 0.00) memory for all participants by the end of the program. The one-way analysis of variance indicated length of stay and participation level were not significant indicators of emotional safety (length of stay: p = 0.91, participation level: p = 0.98) and semantic (length of stay: p = 0.09, participation level: p = 0.60) and procedural (length of stay: p = 0.25, participation level: p = 0.09) memory development. These results suggest psychotherapy incorporating equine interaction was an efficient complementary therapeutic intervention for developing emotional safety and encouraging learning in a young-adult residential addiction treatment program during the COVID-19 pandemic. Full article
(This article belongs to the Special Issue COVID and Post-COVID: The Psychological and Social Impact of COVID-19)
13 pages, 292 KiB  
Review
Observations on the Clinical Features of the Wernicke–Korsakoff Syndrome
by Michael D. Kopelman
J. Clin. Med. 2023, 12(19), 6310; https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm12196310 - 30 Sep 2023
Viewed by 2471
Abstract
This paper begins with a short case report of florid, spontaneous confabulation in a 61-year-old man with an alcohol-induced Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome. His confabulation extended across episodic and personal semantic memory, as well as orientation in time and place, as measured on Dalla Barba’s [...] Read more.
This paper begins with a short case report of florid, spontaneous confabulation in a 61-year-old man with an alcohol-induced Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome. His confabulation extended across episodic and personal semantic memory, as well as orientation in time and place, as measured on Dalla Barba’s Confabulation Battery. Five other brief case summaries will then be presented, followed by a summary of the clinical, neurological, and background neuropsychological findings in three earlier series of Korsakoff patients. These observations will be considered in light of Wijnia’s recent and my own, earlier reviews of the Korsakoff syndrome. Taken together, they indicate the need for a multi-faceted approach (clinical, neurological, neuropsychological, and neuroimaging) to the assessment and diagnosis of the disorder. Full article
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Wernicke's Encephalopathy and Korsakoff's Syndrome)
13 pages, 2078 KiB  
Article
Transcranial Direct-Current Stimulation Improves Verbal Fluency in Children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)
by Vahid Nejati, Reza Estaji and Zahra Helisaz
Brain Sci. 2023, 13(9), 1257; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13091257 - 29 Aug 2023
Cited by 3 | Viewed by 2454
Abstract
Individuals with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) struggle with impaired verbal fluency as an executive function. The left and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) and the right inferior frontal gurus (IFG), which show reduced functionality in individuals with ADHD, are involved in verbal [...] Read more.
Individuals with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) struggle with impaired verbal fluency as an executive function. The left and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) and the right inferior frontal gurus (IFG), which show reduced functionality in individuals with ADHD, are involved in verbal fluency. In this study, a total of thirty-seven children with ADHD participated in two separate experiments. Each experiment included three different stimulation conditions: anodal left dlPFC/cathodal right vmPFC stimulation, the reversed montage, and a sham stimulation in Experiment 1, and anodal right dlPFC, anodal right IFG with extracranial return electrode, and a sham stimulation in Experiment 2. During each session, participants performed semantic and phonemic verbal fluency tasks while receiving tDCS. The results revealed a significant main effect of stimulation condition on phonemic verbal fluency during anodal left dlPFC stimulation in Experiment 1, and on semantic verbal fluency during both real stimulation conditions in Experiment 2. In conclusion, this study suggests that anodal left dlPFC stimulation improves phonemic verbal fluency, while anodal right dlPFC and right IFG stimulation enhance semantic verbal fluency. This domain-specific improvement can be attributed to the distinct cognitive demands of phonemic and semantic verbal fluency tasks. Phonemic verbal fluency heavily relies on working memory processes, whereas semantic verbal fluency requires effective inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility. Full article
(This article belongs to the Section Neurolinguistics)
Show Figures

Figure 1

Figure 1
<p>Experimental procedures. In each study, participants received one of the tDCS protocols in randomized order. The VFT was performed in each session 5 min after stimulation, and the final step was completion of the side-effect questionnaire. Note: VFT: verbal fluency test; LA: left arm.</p>
Full article ">Figure 2
<p>Distribution of electrical field calculated using SimNIBS. Experiment 1: two 5 × 5 cm electrodes were positioned over Fp2 and F3, and the current intensity was set to 2 mA. Experiment 2: two 5 × 5 cm electrodes were positioned over F4 or F8, and the current intensity was set to 2 mA.</p>
Full article ">Figure 3
<p>Shown are the effects of tDCS on the outcome measures. The vertical axis indicates the respective outcome measures of the tasks. The bars show the means, and the error bars represent standard error of means. The horizontal axes show stimulation conditions. The electrode placements followed the 10–20 EEG system, with the abbreviations before and after the slash indicating anodal and cathodal electrode placement, respectively. LA: left arm. *: <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.05, **: <span class="html-italic">p</span> &lt; 0.001.</p>
Full article ">
15 pages, 5002 KiB  
Article
Semantic Priming and Its Link to Verbal Comprehension and Working Memory in Children with Learning Disorders
by Benito Javier Martínez-Briones, Thalía Fernández and Juan Silva-Pereyra
Brain Sci. 2023, 13(7), 1022; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13071022 - 1 Jul 2023
Viewed by 2012
Abstract
Children with learning disorders (LD children) often have heterogeneous cognitive impairments that affect their ability to learn and use basic academic skills. A proposed cause for this variability has been working memory (WM) capacity. Altered patterns of event-related potentials (ERPs) in these children [...] Read more.
Children with learning disorders (LD children) often have heterogeneous cognitive impairments that affect their ability to learn and use basic academic skills. A proposed cause for this variability has been working memory (WM) capacity. Altered patterns of event-related potentials (ERPs) in these children have also been found in the N400 component associated with semantic priming. However, regarding the semantic priming effect in LD children, no distinction has been made for children with varying WM abilities. This study aims to explore the relationship of WM with the brain’s electrophysiological response that underlies semantic priming in LD children that performed a lexical decision task. A total of 40 children (8-10 years old) participated: 28 children with LD and 12 age-matched controls. The ERPs were recorded for each group and analyzed with permutation-based t-tests. The N400 effect was observed only in the control group, and both groups showed a late positive complex (LPC). Permutation-based regression analyses were performed for the results from the LD group using the WISC-IV indices (e.g., Verbal Comprehension and WM) as independent predictors of the ERPs. The Verbal Comprehension Index, but not the WM index, was a significant predictor of the N400 and LPC effects in LD children. Full article
(This article belongs to the Section Neuropsychology)
Show Figures

Figure 1

Figure 1
<p>Venn diagram of the distribution of learning disorders.</p>
Full article ">Figure 2
<p>Experimental flow chart with an illustration of the lexical decision task.</p>
Full article ">Figure 3
<p>ERPs of the control group. The N400 time range (355–550 ms) is represented in the Cz channel in pale gray. The LPC time range (555–800 ms) is represented in dark gray.</p>
Full article ">Figure 4
<p>ERPs of the LD group. The N400 time range (355–550 ms) is represented in the Cz channel in pale gray. The LPC time range (555–800 ms) is represented in dark gray.</p>
Full article ">Figure 5
<p>Statistical maps based on the rho values for the Verbal Comprehension Index as predictor of the N400 and LPC effects in the LD group.</p>
Full article ">
16 pages, 2437 KiB  
Article
The Dynamic Interplay between Loss of Semantic Memory and Semantic Learning Capacity: Insight from Neologisms Learning in Semantic Variant Primary Progressive Aphasia
by Simona Luzzi, Sara Baldinelli, Chiara Fiori, Mauro Morelli and Guido Gainotti
Brain Sci. 2023, 13(5), 788; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13050788 - 11 May 2023
Viewed by 1881
Abstract
Semantic Variant of Primary Progressive Aphasia (svPPA) has often been considered as a loss of knowledge stored in semantic memory, but might also be due to a general disruption of mechanisms allowing the acquisition, storage, and retrieval of semantic memories. In order to [...] Read more.
Semantic Variant of Primary Progressive Aphasia (svPPA) has often been considered as a loss of knowledge stored in semantic memory, but might also be due to a general disruption of mechanisms allowing the acquisition, storage, and retrieval of semantic memories. In order to assess any parallelism in svPPA patients between loss of semantic knowledge and inability to acquire new semantic information, we administered a battery of semantic learning tasks to healthy individuals and svPPA patients, where they were requested to learn new conceptual representations and new word forms, and to associate the former with the latter. A strong relation was found between loss of semantic knowledge and disruption of semantic learning: (a) patients with severe svPPA had the lowest scores in the semantic learning tasks; (b) significant correlations were found between scores obtained in semantic learning tasks and scores obtained in semantic memory disorders in svPPA patients. Full article
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Speech Loss from Dementia? Understanding Aphasia)
Show Figures

Figure 1

Figure 1
<p>The abstract coloured figures and the corresponding neologisms.</p>
Full article ">Figure 2
<p>Post-treatment evaluation: (<b>a</b>) “Provide the novel words associated to novel abstract figures” test; (<b>b</b>) “Novel words to novel abstract figures (abstract distractors)” matching test; (<b>c</b>) “Matching black and white versions of novel abstract figures to arbitrary colours” test; (<b>d</b>) “Novel words to novel abstract figures (concrete distractors)” matching test.</p>
Full article ">Figure 3
<p>Performance of each SD patient just after rehabilitation and at the three-month follow-up. Keys: 1 = “Provide the novel words associated to novel abstract figures” test; 2 = “Novel words to novel abstract figures (abstract distractors)” matching test; 3 = “Matching black and white versions of the novel abstract figures to arbitrary colours” test; 4 = “Novel words to novel abstract figure (concrete distractors)” matching test. Patients 1–8 = mild svPPA; patients 9–11 = moderate svPPA.</p>
Full article ">Figure 4
<p>Three-month follow-up: (<b>a</b>) “Provide the novel words associated to novel abstract figures” test; (<b>b</b>) “Novel words to novel abstract figures (abstract distractors)” matching test; (<b>c</b>) “Matching black and white versions of novel abstract figures to arbitrary colours” test; (<b>d</b>) “Novel words to novel abstract figures (concrete distractors)” matching test.</p>
Full article ">Figure 5
<p>Performance of patients with mild (patients 1, 2, 3) and moderate (patients 4, 5, 6) SD in naming vs. independent free recall of novel words and novel abstract figures.</p>
Full article ">
15 pages, 720 KiB  
Article
Executive Profile of the Logopenic Variant of Primary Progressive Aphasia: Comparison with the Semantic and Non-Fluent Variants and Alzheimer’s Disease
by Sandrine Basaglia-Pappas, Bernard Laurent, Jean-Claude Getenet, Anne Boulangé, Aurelia Rendón de laCruz, Isabelle Simoes Loureiro and Laurent Lefebvre
Brain Sci. 2023, 13(3), 406; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13030406 - 26 Feb 2023
Cited by 5 | Viewed by 3750
Abstract
The logopenic variant of primary progressive aphasia (lvPPA) shows different features from the non-fluent (nfvPPA) and semantic (svPPA) variants of PPA. Although language impairments remain the core symptoms, studies have highlighted the presence of executive disorders at the onset of the disease. Nevertheless, [...] Read more.
The logopenic variant of primary progressive aphasia (lvPPA) shows different features from the non-fluent (nfvPPA) and semantic (svPPA) variants of PPA. Although language impairments remain the core symptoms, studies have highlighted the presence of executive disorders at the onset of the disease. Nevertheless, the results are contradictory, particularly in lvPPA. The aim of this study was to explore the executive profile of lvPPA. We compared executive functioning in lvPPA with the other two variants of PPA, Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and a cognitively healthy group. In total, 70 patients with PPA, 32 patients with AD, and 41 healthy controls were included. They underwent a comprehensive executive battery assessing short-term and working memory, inhibition, flexibility, planning, and initiation. The analyses showed significant differences between the lvPPA group and the control group, except on visuospatial spans and the Stroop test, and between the lvPPA group and the other PPA groups and the AD group for several tasks. Thus, this research highlighted the existence of an executive dysfunction from the onset of the disease in lvPPA but also in the other two variants of PPA. Full article
(This article belongs to the Collection Primary Progressive Aphasia and Apraxia of Speech)
Show Figures

Graphical abstract

Graphical abstract
Full article ">Figure 1
<p>Executive tasks showing statistically significant differences between lvPPA and other clinical groups, inspired by Matias-Guiu et al. [<a href="#B36-brainsci-13-00406" class="html-bibr">36</a>].</p>
Full article ">
12 pages, 755 KiB  
Article
The Relationship between Neuropsychiatric Symptoms and Cognitive Performance in Older Adults with Normal Cognition
by Ioannis Liampas, Vasileios Siokas, Constantine George Lyketsos and Efthimios Dardiotis
Medicina 2022, 58(11), 1586; https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina58111586 - 3 Nov 2022
Cited by 14 | Viewed by 2399
Abstract
Background and Objectives: To explore whether specific Neuropsychiatric Symptoms (NPS) are related to worse performance in particular cognitive domains. Materials and Methods: A cross-sectional analysis of the baseline evaluations of older (≥60 years), cognitively unimpaired (CU) participants from the National Alzheimer’s [...] Read more.
Background and Objectives: To explore whether specific Neuropsychiatric Symptoms (NPS) are related to worse performance in particular cognitive domains. Materials and Methods: A cross-sectional analysis of the baseline evaluations of older (≥60 years), cognitively unimpaired (CU) participants from the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC) Uniform Data Set was performed. Data were derived from 43 Alzheimer’s Disease Research Centers. Cognitively impaired individuals, participants with psychiatric disorders and/or under treatment with antipsychotic, anxiolytic, sedative, or hypnotic agents were excluded. NPS were assessed using the Neuropsychiatric Inventory Questionnaire. The association of NPS with participants’ performance on episodic memory, semantic memory, language, attention, processing speed and executive function was analysed using an adjusted (considering important demographic and medical factors) multivariate general linear model. Results: A total of 7179 CU, older, predominantly female, Caucasian, and well-educated participants were included in the present analysis. Among them, 1856 individuals had one or more NPS. Our analysis revealed that moderate/severe anxiety was related to worse performance on semantic memory, attention and executive function, the presence of hallucinations was linked to worse processing speed and executive function scores, while the presence of elation/euphoria and aberrant motor behaviour were associated with poorer attention and language performance, respectively. In the context of a secondary, exploratory analysis, the presence of moderate/severe delusions was related to worse processing speed and executive function performance. Conclusions: The relationship between specific NPS and worse performance in particular cognitive domains could inform the formulation of individualized preventive strategies directed to the ‘‘fortification’’ of specific cognitive functions in CU individuals with NPS. Full article
Show Figures

Figure 1

Figure 1
<p>Flowchart of participant selection.</p>
Full article ">
10 pages, 627 KiB  
Article
How Has COVID-19 Impacted Our Language Use?
by Francesca Pisano, Alessio Manfredini, Daniela Brachi, Luana Landi, Lucia Sorrentino, Marianna Bottone, Chiara Incoccia and Paola Marangolo
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(21), 13836; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192113836 - 24 Oct 2022
Cited by 4 | Viewed by 1917
Abstract
The COVID-19 pandemic has led to severe consequences for people’s mental health. The pandemic has also influenced our language use, shaping our word formation habits. The overuse of new metaphorical meanings has received particular attention from the media. Here, we wanted to investigate [...] Read more.
The COVID-19 pandemic has led to severe consequences for people’s mental health. The pandemic has also influenced our language use, shaping our word formation habits. The overuse of new metaphorical meanings has received particular attention from the media. Here, we wanted to investigate whether these metaphors have led to the formation of new semantic associations in memory. A sample of 120 university students was asked to decide whether a target word was or was not related to a prime stimulus. Responses for pandemic pairs in which the target referred to the newly acquired metaphorical meaning of the prime (i.e., “trench”—“hospital”) were compared to pre-existing semantically related pairs (i.e., “trench”—“soldier”) and neutral pairs (i.e., “trench”—“response”). Results revealed greater accuracy and faster response times for pandemic pairs than for semantic pairs and for semantic pairs compared to neutral ones. These findings suggest that the newly learned pandemic associations have created stronger semantic links in our memory compared to the pre-existing ones. Thus, this work confirms the adaptive nature of human language, and it underlines how the overuse of metaphors evoking dramatic images has been, in part, responsible for many psychological disorders still reported among people nowadays. Full article
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Impact of Coronavirus (COVID-19))
Show Figures

Figure 1

Figure 1
<p>Mean percentage of response accuracy for pandemic, semantic, and neutral stimuli (*** <italic>p</italic> ≤ 0.001, * <italic>p</italic> ≤ 0.05).</p>
Full article ">Figure 2
<p>Mean percentage of reaction times (RTs) for pandemic, semantic, and neutral stimuli (*** <italic>p</italic> ≤ 0.001).</p>
Full article ">
11 pages, 282 KiB  
Article
Association between Motor Signs and Cognitive Performance in Cognitively Unimpaired Older Adults: A Cross-Sectional Study Using the NACC Database
by Vasileios Siokas, Ioannis Liampas, Constantine G. Lyketsos and Efthimios Dardiotis
Brain Sci. 2022, 12(10), 1365; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12101365 - 8 Oct 2022
Cited by 10 | Viewed by 1951
Abstract
Aiming to examine whether specific motor signs are associated with worse performance in specific cognitive domains among cognitively unimpaired (CU) individuals, we performed a cross-sectional analysis of data from the baseline evaluations of older, CU participants from the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC) [...] Read more.
Aiming to examine whether specific motor signs are associated with worse performance in specific cognitive domains among cognitively unimpaired (CU) individuals, we performed a cross-sectional analysis of data from the baseline evaluations of older, CU participants from the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC) Uniform Data Set. In total, 8149 CU (≥60 years) participants were included. Of these, 905 individuals scored ≥ 2 on at least one of the motor domains of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale part III (UPDRSIII). Cognitively impaired individuals, participants with psychiatric disorders and/or under treatment with antipsychotic, anxiolytic, sedative or hypnotic agents were excluded. Nine motor signs were examined: hypophonia, masked facies, resting tremor, action/postural tremor, rigidity, bradykinesia, impaired chair rise, impaired posture/gait and postural instability. Their association with performance on episodic memory, semantic memory, language, attention, processing speed or executive function was assessed using crude and adjusted linear regression models. Individuals with impaired chair rise had worse episodic memory, semantic memory, processing speed and executive function, while those with bradykinesia had worse language, processing speed and executive function. Sensitivity analyses, by excluding participants with cerebrovascular disease or PD, or other Parkinsonism, produced similar results with the exception of the relationship between bradykinesia and language performance. Full article
4 pages, 2451 KiB  
Comment
Entorhinal Cortex and Persistent Olfactory Loss in COVID-19 Patients: A Neuroanatomical Hypothesis. Comment on Fiorentino et al. Correlations between Persistent Olfactory and Semantic Memory Disorders after SARS-CoV-2 Infection. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 714
by Pietro De Luca, Pasquale Marra, Ignazio La Mantia, Francesco Antonio Salzano, Angelo Camaioni and Arianna Di Stadio
Brain Sci. 2022, 12(7), 850; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12070850 - 29 Jun 2022
Cited by 5 | Viewed by 4445
Abstract
Recently, Fiorentino et al. [...] Full article
Show Figures

Figure 1

Figure 1
<p>Anatomical visualization of olfactory pathways and mnemonic connections, with a central role of the entorhinal cortex.</p>
Full article ">
17 pages, 2446 KiB  
Article
TR-Net: A Transformer-Based Neural Network for Point Cloud Processing
by Luyao Liu, Enqing Chen and Yingqiang Ding
Machines 2022, 10(7), 517; https://doi.org/10.3390/machines10070517 - 27 Jun 2022
Cited by 5 | Viewed by 5067
Abstract
Point cloud is a versatile geometric representation that could be applied in computer vision tasks. On account of the disorder of point cloud, it is challenging to design a deep neural network used in point cloud analysis. Furthermore, most existing frameworks for point [...] Read more.
Point cloud is a versatile geometric representation that could be applied in computer vision tasks. On account of the disorder of point cloud, it is challenging to design a deep neural network used in point cloud analysis. Furthermore, most existing frameworks for point cloud processing either hardly consider the local neighboring information or ignore context-aware and spatially-aware features. To deal with the above problems, we propose a novel point cloud processing architecture named TR-Net, which is based on transformer. This architecture reformulates the point cloud processing task as a set-to-set translation problem. TR-Net directly operates on raw point clouds without any data transformation or annotation, which reduces the consumption of computing resources and memory usage. Firstly, a neighborhood embedding backbone is designed to effectively extract the local neighboring information from point cloud. Then, an attention-based sub-network is constructed to better learn a semantically abundant and discriminatory representation from embedded features. Finally, effective global features are yielded through feeding the features extracted by attention-based sub-network into a residual backbone. For different downstream tasks, we build different decoders. Extensive experiments on the public datasets illustrate that our approach outperforms other state-of-the-art methods. For example, our TR-Net performs 93.1% overall accuracy on the ModelNet40 dataset and the TR-Net archives a mIou of 85.3% on the ShapeNet dataset for part segmentation. Full article
(This article belongs to the Topic Intelligent Systems and Robotics)
Show Figures

Figure 1

Figure 1
<p>The architecture of TR-Net. The decoder of TR-Net consists of classification (top branch) and segmentation (bottom branch). <span class="html-italic">C</span> means concatenate operator. FCBR consists of fully connected layer, batch normalization and ReLU. FCBRD stands FCBR followed by a Dropout layer with rate 0.5.</p>
Full article ">Figure 2
<p>(<b>a</b>) is the description of offset-attention. (<b>b</b>) is the structure of attention-based sub-network.</p>
Full article ">Figure 3
<p>The architecture of residual backbone. BLM combines BachNorm, LeakyReLU and MaxPooling layers. BR combines BachNorm and ReLU layers.</p>
Full article ">Figure 4
<p>The visualization of object part segmentation results on the ShapeNet dataset. GT denotes Ground Truth.</p>
Full article ">Figure 5
<p>Six run results (OA) of TR-Net with different neighbors on ModelNet40 test dataset. We use <span class="html-italic">k</span> to denote the number of neighborhood point sets sampled for each point.</p>
Full article ">Figure 6
<p>The loss of TR-Net with different neighbors during each testing phase.</p>
Full article ">
12 pages, 328 KiB  
Article
Influence of Menstrual Cycle Length and Age at Menarche on Symptoms, Cognition, Social Cognition, and Metacognition in Patients with First-Episode Psychosis
by Paula Barrau-Sastre, Irene Birulés, Marina Verdaguer-Rodríguez, Raquel López-Carrilero, Marta Ferrer-Quintero, Helena García-Mieres, Luciana Díaz-Cutraro, Eva Grasa, Esther Pousa, Ester Lorente, Trinidad Peláez, María Luisa Barrigón, Isabel Ruiz-Delgado, Fermín González-Higueras, Jordi Cid, Alfonso Gutiérrez-Zotes, Daniel Cuadras, Judith Usall, Regina Vila-Badia, Ana Barajas, Susana Ochoa and on behalf of the Spanish Metacognition Groupadd Show full author list remove Hide full author list
Women 2022, 2(2), 135-146; https://doi.org/10.3390/women2020015 - 2 Jun 2022
Cited by 2 | Viewed by 2886
Abstract
A protective effect has traditionally been attributed to estrogen in psychotic disorders. The aim of this study was to investigate cumulative lifetime estrogen by assessing the menstrual cycle length, age at menarche, and years of difference between the onset of psychotic symptoms and [...] Read more.
A protective effect has traditionally been attributed to estrogen in psychotic disorders. The aim of this study was to investigate cumulative lifetime estrogen by assessing the menstrual cycle length, age at menarche, and years of difference between the onset of psychotic symptoms and the age of menarche, measuring their effects on symptoms, cognition, social cognition, and metacognition. As it was not possible to directly measure cumulative estrogen levels over the lifetime of a patient, the study sample was composed of 42 women with first-episode psychosis; estrogen levels were inferred by the menstrual cycle length, age at menarche, and years of difference between the onset of psychotic symptoms and menarche. All patients were assessed with a battery of questionnaires using the BDI, PSYRATS, PANSS, STROOP, TAVEC, WSCT, IPSAQ, and BCIS questionnaires. The results related to menstrual cycle length showed a relationship with memory; specifically, shorter cycles with semantic strategies (p = 0.046) and longer cycles with serial strategies in the short term (p = 0.005) as well as in the long term (p = 0.031). The results also showed a relationship with perseverative errors (p = 0.035) and self-certainty (p = 0.049). Only personalized bias (p = 0.030) was found to be significant in relation to the age at menarche. When analyzing the differences in years of difference between the age at menarche and the onset of psychotic symptoms, the results indicated lower scores in women with a smaller difference between both events in memory (short-term (p = 0.050), long-term (p = 0.024), intrusions (p = 0.013), and recognition (p = 0.043)) and non-perseverative errors (p = 0.024). No relationship was found between symptoms and menstrual characteristics. The investigatory outcomes seem to indicate a relationship between estrogen cumulative effects and the memory domain. More in-depth investigations in the field are necessary in order to improve personalized treatment in women with psychosis. Full article
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Feature Papers in Women 2021)
Back to TopTop