Eyes on the Pupil Size: Pupillary Response During Sentence Processing in Aphasia
<p>Example of the visual world display and a sample experimental sentence.</p> "> Figure 2
<p>An example of the experimental procedure. Each trial would begin with a fixation cross, followed by a blank screen, and then the 2 × 2 image display. Following the onset of the visual display, sentences were presented over headphones while participant eye gaze and pupillary data were collected. At the end of each trial, participants were asked a comprehension question to ensure they were attending to the auditory information.</p> "> Figure 3
<p>Time windows of interest. Time Window 1 includes the whole sentence. Time Window 2 is from the beginning of the sentence until the offset of NP2.</p> "> Figure 4
<p>Time Window 1. Pupil responses throughout the whole sentence for AMC (blue) and IWA (red). Data are indicated by the shaded ribbons and growth curve models are indicated by the solid line.</p> "> Figure 5
<p>Time Window 2. Pupil responses from the beginning of the sentence to the onset of the second noun. AMC data are graphed on the left, IWA data on the right. The biased adjective condition for each group is shown in green, and the unbiased adjective condition is shown in grey. Data are indicated by the shaded ribbons and growth curve models are indicated by the solid line. Dotted lines were manually inserted to demonstrate visual differences in linear fit.</p> ">
Abstract
:1. Introduction
object | subject | verb |
|
- (2a)
- The eagle saw the venomous snakei that the bear cautiously encounteredi underneath the narrow bridge.
- (2b)
- The eagle saw the voracious snakei that the bear cautiously encounteredi underneath the narrow bridge.
1.1. Pupillary Measures of Cognitive Load During Sentence Processing
1.2. Pupillometry in Aphasia
1.3. Present Study
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Stimuli
2.3. Procedure
2.4. Preprocessing
2.4.1. Data Cleaning
2.4.2. Data Loss
2.4.3. Time Windows for Analysis
2.4.4. Growth Curve Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Comprehension
3.2. Pupil Dilation Response Related to Group Effects—Time Window 1
3.3. Pupil Dilation Response Related to Semantic Biasing—Time Window 2
4. Discussion
4.1. Processing Effort During Complex Non-Canonical Sentence Processing
4.2. Effect of Semantic Biasing on Noun-Processing Effort
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Unbiased Adjective | Biased Adjective |
---|---|
The duck followed the perfect kitten that the cow deliberately nudged across the grassy meadow. | The duck followed the playful kitten that the cow deliberately nudged across the grassy meadow. |
The veterinarian greeted the popular king that the criminal mistakenly expected at the stunningly lavish gala. | The veterinarian greeted the powerful king that the criminal mistakenly expected at the stunningly lavish gala. |
The scorpion annoyed the anxious bull that the bee constantly pestered in the abandoned railroad yard. | The scorpion annoyed the angry bull that the bee constantly pestered in the abandoned railroad yard. |
The crocodile spied the weird owl that the chameleon momentarily faced in the exotic animal show. | The crocodile spied the wise owl that the chameleon momentarily faced during the exotic animal show. |
The crab helped the coy puppy that the rabbit relentlessly teased before playful tussle. | The crab helped the cute puppy that the rabbit relentlessly teased before the playful tussle. |
The lawyer visited the forgetful gymnast that the butler allegedly helped with the illegal cover-up. | The lawyer visited the flexible gymnast that the butler allegedly helped with the illegal cover-up. |
The magician passed the redheaded nun that the mailman compassionately soothed after the traumatic event. | The magician passed the religious nun that the mailman compassionately soothed after the traumatic event. |
The ladybug observed the smelly bat that the opossum deliberately avoided near the historic monument. | The ladybug observed the scary bat that the opossum deliberately avoided near the historic monument. |
The astronaut approached the sad jockey that the salesman incorrectly judged throughout the dinner party. | The astronaut approached the short jockey that the salesman incorrectly judged throughout the dinner party. |
The otter spotted the shiny octopus that seagull unsurprisingly smelled after the hot and sunny day. | The otter spotted the slimy octopus that seagull unsurprisingly smelled after the hot and sunny day. |
The deer noticed the male gorilla that the hummingbird thoroughly amused with the acrobatic display. | The deer noticed the mean gorilla that the hummingbird thoroughly amused with the acrobatic display. |
The ostrich recognized the delightful toucan that the baboon hesitantly touched during the bizarre encounter. | The ostrich recognized the colorful toucan that the baboon hesitantly touched during the bizarre encounter. |
The spider scared the live rooster that the porcupine accidentally bumped on the side of the country road. | The spider scared the loud rooster that the porcupine accidentally bumped on the side of the country road. |
The dentist helped the tired maid that the plumber heartlessly cheated in spite of the cautious investment. | The dentist helped the tidy maid that the plumber heartlessly cheated in spite of the cautious investment. |
The orangutan examined the defenseless cockroach that the parrot quickly located near the bottom of the staircase. | The orangutan examined the disgusting cockroach that the parrot quickly located near the bottom of the staircase. |
The parrot examined the pretty dove that the orangutan casually shoved off of the exam table. | The parrot examined the peaceful dove that the orangutan casually shoved off of the exam table. |
The salesman approached the slim wrestler that the astronaut unconditionally loved in spite of the tabloid gossip. | The salesman approached the strong wrestler that the astronaut unconditionally loved in spite of the tabloid gossip. |
The baboon recognized the shy tortoise that the ostrich understandably scared with the unexpected noise. | The baboon recognized the slow tortoise that the ostrich understandably scared with the unexpected noise. |
The chameleon spied the plump flamingo that the crocodile instinctively grabbed during the calculated ambush. | The chameleon spied the pink flamingo that the crocodile instinctively grabbed during the calculated ambush. |
The bee annoyed the miniature unicorn that the woodpecker distinctively heard at the end of the fairy tale. | The bee annoyed the magical unicorn that the woodpecker distinctively heard at the end of the fairy tale. |
The porcupine scared the puny skunk that the spider unexpectedly bit under the deck in the back yard | The porcupine scared the putrid skunk that the spider unexpectedly bit under the deck in the back yard. |
The mailman visited the humorous queen that the magician easily confused in the middle of the fundraiser. | The mailman visited the homecoming queen that the magician easily confused in the middle of the fundraiser. |
The opossum observed the grimy swan that the ladybug lazily circled in the pond at the city park. | The opossum observed the graceful swan that the ladybug lazily circled in the pond at the city park. |
The rabbit helped the sleepy rat that the crab repeatedly pinched during the minor scuffle. | The rabbit helped the sneaky rat that the crab repeatedly pinched during the minor scuffle. |
The butler visited the happy prince that the lawyer properly instructed in the art of negotiation. | The butler visited the handsome prince that the lawyer properly instructed in the art of negotiation. |
The criminal greeted the gabby mechanic that the veterinarian angrily informed of the fabricated charges. | The criminal greeted the greasy mechanic that the veterinarian angrily informed of the fabricated charges. |
The plumber helped the easygoing cheerleader that the dentist recently met before the city council meeting. | The plumber helped the energetic cheerleader that the dentist recently met before the city council meeting. |
The hummingbird noticed the fake cheetah that the deer cautiously examined next to the perfectly clear lake. | The hummingbird noticed the fast cheetah that the deer cautiously examined next to the perfectly clear lake. |
The seagull spotted the incredible dolphin that the otter eventually found in amongst the dense kelp forest. | The seagull spotted the intelligent dolphin that the otter eventually found in amongst the dense kelp forest. |
The cow followed the dizzy pig that the duck confidently led along the winding country road. | The cow followed the dirty pig that the duck confidently led along the winding country road. |
References
- Engel, S.; Shapiro, L.P.; Love, T. Proform-Antecedent linking in individuals with agrammatic aphasia: A test of the Intervener Hypothesis. J. Neurolinguist. 2018, 45, 79–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Prather, P.; Zurif, E.; Love, T.; Brownell, H. Speed of Lexical Activation in Nonfluent Broca’s Aphasia and Fluent Wernicke’s Aphasia. Brain Lang. 1997, 59, 391–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ferrill, M.; Love, T.; Walenski, M.; Shapiro, L.P. The Time-Course of Lexical Activation During Sentence Comprehension in People With Aphasia. Am. J. Speech-Lang. Pathol. 2012, 21, S179–S189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Love, T.; Swinney, D.; Walenski, M.; Zurif, E. How left inferior frontal cortex participates in syntactic processing: Evidence from aphasia. Brain Lang. 2008, 107, 203–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burkhardt, P.; Piñango, M.M.; Wong, K. The role of the anterior left hemisphere in real-time sentence comprehension: Evidence from split intransitivity. Brain Lang. 2003, 86, 9–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Swinney, D.; Prather, P.; Love, T. The time-course of lexical access and the role of context: Converging evidence from normal and aphasic processing. In Language and the Brain; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2000; pp. 273–292. [Google Scholar]
- Prather, P.; Shapiro, L.; Zurif, E.; Swinney, D. Real-time examinations of lexical processing in aphasics. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 1991, 20, 271–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Swinney, D.; Zurif, E.; Prather, P.; Love, T. Neurological distribution of processing resources underlying language comprehension. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1996, 8, 174–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Swaab, T. Event-related potentials in cognitive neuropsychology: Methodological considerations and an example from studies of aphasia. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 1998, 30, 157–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Swaab, T.; Brown, C.; Hagoort, P. Spoken Sentence Comprehension in Aphasia: Event-related Potential Evidence for a Lexical Integration Deficit. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1997, 9, 39–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thompson, C.K.; Choy, J.J. Pronominal Resolution and Gap Filling in Agrammatic Aphasia: Evidence from Eye Movements. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 2009, 38, 255–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Meyer, A.M.; Mack, J.E.; Thompson, C.K. Tracking passive sentence comprehension in agrammatic aphasia. J. Neurolinguist. 2012, 25, 31–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Akhavan, N.; Sen, C.; Baker, C.; Abbott, N.; Gravier, M.; Love, T. Effect of Lexical-Semantic Cues during Real-Time Sentence Processing in Aphasia. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baker, C.; Love, T. The effect of time on lexical and syntactic processing in aphasia. J. Neurolinguist. 2023, 67, 101142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baker, C.; Love, T. Modulating Complex Sentence Processing in Aphasia Through Attention and Semantic Networks. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2023, 66, 5011–5035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Laeng, B.; Sirois, S.; Gredebäck, G. Pupillometry: A Window to the Preconscious? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 7, 18–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Poe, G.R.; Foote, S.; Eschenko, O.; Johansen, J.P.; Bouret, S.; Aston-Jones, G.; Harley, C.W.; Manahan-Vaughan, D.; Weinshenker, D.; Valentino, R. Locus coeruleus: A new look at the blue spot. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2020, 21, 644–659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chapman, L.R.; Hallowell, B. The Unfolding of Cognitive Effort During Sentence Processing: Pupillometric Evidence From People With and Without Aphasia. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2021, 64, 4900–4917. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Engelhardt, P.E.; Ferreira, F.; Patsenko, E.G. Pupillometry reveals processing load during spoken language comprehension. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2010, 63, 639–645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahneman, D.; Tversky, A. On the psychology of prediction. Psychol. Rev. 1973, 80, 237–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van Rij, J.; Hendriks, P.; van Rijn, H.; Baayen, R.H.; Wood, S.N. Analyzing the Time Course of Pupillometric Data. Trends Hear. 2019, 23, 2331216519832483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chapman, L.R.; Hallowell, B. Expecting Questions Modulates Cognitive Effort in a Syntactic Processing Task: Evidence From Pupillometry. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2021, 64, 121–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ben-Nun, Y. The use of pupillometry in the study of on-line verbal processing: Evidence for depths of processing. Brain Lang. 1986, 28, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Aydın, Ö.; Uzun, İ.P. Pupil Dilation Response to Prosody and Syntax During Auditory Sentence Processing. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 2023, 52, 153–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Demberg, V.; Sayeed, A. The Frequency of Rapid Pupil Dilations as a Measure of Linguistic Processing Difficulty. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0146194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Geller, J.; Winn, M.B.; Mahr, T.; Mirman, D. GazeR: A Package for Processing Gaze Position and Pupil Size Data. Behav. Res. Methods 2020, 52, 2232–2255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McGarrigle, R.; Knight, S.; Rakusen, L.; Geller, J.; Mattys, S. Older adults show a more sustained pattern of effortful listening than young adults. Psychol. Aging 2021, 36, 504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chapman, L.R.; Hallowell, B. A Novel Pupillometric Method for Indexing Word Difficulty in Individuals With and Without Aphasia. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2015, 58, 1508–1520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, E.S.; Suleman, S.; Hopper, T. Cognitive effort during a short-term memory (STM) task in individuals with aphasia. J. Neurolinguist. 2018, 48, 190–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Suleman, S.; Kim, E.S.; Hopper, T. A comparison of processing load during non-verbal decision-making in two individuals with aphasia. In Proceedings of the Front. Psychol. Conference Abstract: Academy of Aphasia 53rd Annual Meeting, Tucson, AZ, USA, 18–20 October 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Hofmeister, P. Representational complexity and memory retrieval in language comprehension. Lang. Cogn. Process. 2011, 26, 376–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goodglass, H.; Kaplan, E.; Barresi, B. The Assessment of Aphasia and Related Disorders; Lippincott Williams & Wilkins: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Kertesz, A. Western Aphasia Battery-Revised; The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Love, T.; Oster, E. On the Categorization of Aphasic Typologies: The SOAP (A Test of Syntactic Complexity). J. Psycholinguist. Res. 2002, 31, 503–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Psychology Software Tools. E-Prime, version 3.0; Psychology Software Tools: Sharpsburg, PA, USA, 2016; Available online: https://support.pstnet.com/ (accessed on 20 January 2025).
- R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2022; Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 19 May 2021).
- Jessen, S.; Altvater-Mackensen, N.; Grossmann, T. Pupillary responses reveal infants’ discrimination of facial emotions independent of conscious perception. Cognition 2016, 150, 163–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Reilly, J.; Kelly, A.; Kim, S.H.; Jett, S.; Zuckerman, B. The human task-evoked pupillary response function is linear: Implications for baseline response scaling in pupillometry. Behav. Res. Methods 2019, 51, 865–878. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mathôt, S.; Fabius, J.; Van Heusden, E.; Van der Stigchel, S. Safe and sensible preprocessing and baseline correction of pupil-size data. Behav. Res. Methods 2018, 50, 94–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Winn, M.B.; Wendt, D.; Koelewijn, T.; Kuchinsky, S.E. Best Practices and Advice for Using Pupillometry to Measure Listening Effort: An Introduction for Those Who Want to Get Started. Trends Hear. 2018, 22, 2331216518800869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mirman, D. Growth Curve Analysis and Visualization; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Mirman, D.; Dixon, J.A.; Manguson, J.S. Statistical and computational models of the visual world paradigm: Growth curves and individual differences. J. Mem. Lang. 2008, 59, 475–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuchinsky, S.E.; Ahlstrom, J.B.; Vaden, K.I., Jr.; Cute, S.L.; Humes, L.E.; Dubno, J.R.; Eckert, M.A. Pupil size varies with word listening and response selection difficulty in older adults with hearing loss. Psychophysiology 2013, 50, 23–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bates, D.; Maechler, M.; Bolker, B.; Walker, S.; Christensen RH, B.; Singmann, H.; Dai, B.; Grothendieck, G.; Green, P.; Bolker, M.B. Package ‘lme4’. Convergence 2015, 12, 2. [Google Scholar]
- Geller, J.; Landrigan, J.-F.; Mirman, D. A Pupillometric Examination of Cognitive Control in Taxonomic and Thematic Semantic Memory. J. Cogn. 2019, 2, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Peavler, W.S. Individual differences in pupil size and performance. In Pupillary Dynamics and Behavior; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1974; pp. 159–175. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, E.L.; Miller Singley, A.T.; Peckham, A.D.; Johnson, S.L.; Bunge, S.A. Task-evoked pupillometry provides a window into the development of short-term memory capacity. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hossain, G.; Yeasin, M. Understanding effects of cognitive load from pupillary responses using hilbert analytic phase. In Proceedings of the IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition Workshops, Columbus, OH, USA, 23–28 June 2014; pp. 375–380. [Google Scholar]
- van der Wel, P.; van Steenbergen, H. Pupil dilation as an index of effort in cognitive control tasks: A review. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2018, 25, 2005–2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuchinke, L.; Võ, M.L.-H.; Hofmann, M.; Jacobs, A.M. Pupillary responses during lexical decisions vary with word frequency but not emotional valence. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2007, 65, 132–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Papesh, M.H.; Goldinger, S.D. Pupil-BLAH-metry: Cognitive effort in speech planning reflected by pupil dilation. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2012, 74, 754–765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schmidtke, J. Second language experience modulates word retrieval effort in bilinguals: Evidence from pupillometry. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Guasch, M.; Ferré, P.; Haro, J. Pupil dilation is sensitive to the cognate status of words: Further evidence for non-selectivity in bilingual lexical access. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2017, 20, 49–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rojas, C.; Vega-Rodríguez, Y.E.; Lagos, G.; Cabrera-Miguieles, M.G.; Sandoval, Y.; Crisosto-Alarcón, J. Applicability and usefulness of pupillometry in the study of lexical access. A scoping review of primary research. Front. Psychol. 2024, 15, 1372912. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
IWA | Sex | Years Post-Stroke | Age at Testing | Years of Education | Lesion Location | BDAE-3 Severity Level | WAB-AQ | SOAP-SR (%) | SOAP-OR (%) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
009 | M | 15 | 55 | 17 | L IFG (BA44 and BA45) and PFG w/subcortical extension; w/sparing of the occipital lobe | 4 | 67.7 | 60 | 40 |
017 | M | 18 | 66 | 15 | L ACA and MCA infarct | 4 | 95.4 | 100 | 90 |
101 | M | 9 | 67 | 20 | L IFG (BA44) and PFG w/subcortical extension; w/sparing of the occipital lobe | 2 | 82.6 | 100 | 30 |
130 | M | 8 | 63 | 16 | L IPL w/posterior extension sparing STG | 4 | 90.5 | 75 | 55 |
140 | F | 16 | 42 | 16 | L MCA infarct | 2 | 75.7 | 80 | 30 |
151 | F | 7 | 65 | 16 | L MCA infarct w/subcortical extension | 4 | 95.8 | 100 | 100 |
159 | F | 6 | 64 | 16 | L MCA infarct | 3 | 92.4 | 100 | 70 |
165 | F | 4 | 64 | 12 | L MCA infarct | 3 | ND | 80 | 60 |
169 | M | 4 | 59 | 12 | L MCA infarct | 2 | 28.2 | 80 | 40 |
190 | F | 6 | 76 | 12 | L STG | 3 | 88.2 | 90 | 40 |
191 | M | 1 | 57 | 16 | L MCA infarct | 4.5 | 98.4 | 100 | 60 |
Semantic Condition | Example Sentence |
---|---|
Biased adjective | The eagle saw the venomous snakei that the bear cautiously encounteredi underneath the narrow bridge. |
Unbiased adjective | The eagle saw the voracious snakei that the bear cautiously encounteredi underneath the narrow bridge. |
Data Fit Term | What the Term Represents |
---|---|
Intercept | Average pupil size across the time-window |
Linear | Constant rate of change over time (i.e., slope) |
Quadratic | Second-order orthogonal polynomial. The rate of increase (rise) or decrease (fall) around the primary curve inflection point |
Cubic | Third-order orthogonal polynomial. The extent to which there is a secondary inflection point in the response (positive values indicate that the pupil response had an earlier rise and fall, whereas a negative value indicates a later peak) |
Control | IWA | |
---|---|---|
Biased | 77.8% | 60.6% |
Unbiased | 79.3% | 61.4% |
Model fit | |||
χ2 | p | ||
Base | |||
Group | 11.40 | 0.02 | |
Group × Condition | 4931.92 | <0.001 | |
Group effect | |||
β | t | p | |
Intercept | 0.003 | 0.119 | 0.907 |
Linear | −0.127 | −1.199 | 0.248 |
Quadratic | −0.104 | −2.578 | 0.020 |
Cubic | 0.07 | 2.782 | 0.013 |
Group × Condition effect | |||
β | t | p | |
Intercept | 0.009 | 0.589 | 0.564 |
Linear | 0.099 | 2.362 | 0.031 |
Quadratic | 0.018 | 0.337 | 0.741 |
Cubic | −0.005 | −0.118 | 0.907 |
Model fit | ||||||
AMC | IWA | |||||
χ2 | p | χ2 | p | |||
Base | ||||||
Condition | 15.07 | 0.002 | 57.882 | <0.001 | ||
Condition effect | ||||||
AMC | IWA | |||||
β | t | p | β | t | p | |
Intercept | 0.009 | 0.794 | 0.431 | 0.004 | 0.600 | 0.551 |
Linear | 0.011 | 2.071 | 0.038 | 0.031 | 7.229 | <0.001 |
Quadratic | 0.016 | 3.049 | 0.002 | 0.008 | 1.895 | 0.058 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Sen, C.; Abbott, N.; Akhavan, N.; Baker, C.; Love, T. Eyes on the Pupil Size: Pupillary Response During Sentence Processing in Aphasia. Brain Sci. 2025, 15, 107. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15020107
Sen C, Abbott N, Akhavan N, Baker C, Love T. Eyes on the Pupil Size: Pupillary Response During Sentence Processing in Aphasia. Brain Sciences. 2025; 15(2):107. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15020107
Chicago/Turabian StyleSen, Christina, Noelle Abbott, Niloofar Akhavan, Carolyn Baker, and Tracy Love. 2025. "Eyes on the Pupil Size: Pupillary Response During Sentence Processing in Aphasia" Brain Sciences 15, no. 2: 107. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15020107
APA StyleSen, C., Abbott, N., Akhavan, N., Baker, C., & Love, T. (2025). Eyes on the Pupil Size: Pupillary Response During Sentence Processing in Aphasia. Brain Sciences, 15(2), 107. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15020107