Short-Term Reproduction of Active Movement with Visual Feedback and Passive Movement with a Therapist’s Hands
<p>Experimental setup. A participant maintains standing over the support surface of the gravicorder, and a therapist stands behind him. A monitor is placed in front of the participant and therapist. The participant wears shutter goggles that occlude their vision temporarily. COP—center of pressure.</p> "> Figure 2
<p>The schedule of the target movements and COP cursor on the monitor. Display (<b>A</b>) and cursor indicating target COP and cursor indicating online COP position (<b>B</b>). A participant moves his own pelvis (active movement instruction) or a therapist moves the participant’s pelvis (passive movement instruction) so that the COP cursor is at the target in the instruction phase, and the participant reproduces the instructed pelvis movement in the reproduction phase (<b>A</b>). A target appears at 1 of 16 sites (1st target) and then appears at another site (2nd target) in the instruction phase (<b>B</b>). Arrows indicate movement of a COP cursor.</p> "> Figure 3
<p>Example of COPx traces in the instruction phase. Two traces are similar in the instruction phase. PM—passive movement instructional method; AM—active movement instructional method.</p> "> Figure 4
<p>Example of COPx traces in the instruction and reproduction phases of the passive movement instruction condition. For the 2nd target, a large difference in the traces between the instruction and reproduction phases is present, whereas it is not for the 1st target. Arrows indicate movement of a COP cursor.</p> "> Figure 5
<p>Simple main effect on the AE in the COPx. The simple main effects of the instruction condition and retention interval (<b>A</b>) and that of the target distance and retention interval (<b>B</b>) are shown. Bars indicate the mean, and error bars indicate the standard error of the mean. Asterisks indicate a significant difference (<span class="html-italic">p</span> < 0.05). AM: active movement, PM: passive movement, and AE: absolute error.</p> "> Figure 6
<p>Main effect of the instructional method (<b>A</b>) and simple main effects of the Target Period and Target Distance (<b>B</b>) on the AE of COPy. Bars indicate the mean, and error bars indicate the standard error of the mean. Asterisks indicate a significant main effect (<b>A</b>) or simple main effects (<b>B</b>) (<span class="html-italic">p</span> < 0.05). AM: active movement, PM: passive movement, and AE: absolute error.</p> "> Figure 7
<p>Main effects on the CE. Bars indicate the mean, and error bars indicate the standard error of the mean. Asterisks indicate a significant main effect (<span class="html-italic">p</span> < 0.05). AM: active movement, PM: passive movement, and CE: constant error.</p> ">
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Apparatus
2.3. Target and COP Cursor
2.4. Sensitivity of COP Cursor
2.5. Procedure
2.6. Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. COP Differences in the Instruction Phase
3.2. Reproduction Error in the Medial–Lateral Movement Axis
3.3. Reproduction Error in the Anterior–Posterior Movement Axis
3.4. Reproduction Bias
4. Discussion
4.1. Reproduction Error
4.2. Target Periods
4.3. Reproduction Bias
4.4. Clinical Implications
4.5. Limitations and Directions for Further Research
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Beets, I.A.; Macé, M.; Meesen, R.L.; Cuypers, K.; Levin, O.; Swinnen, S.P. Active versus passive training of a complex bimanual task: Is prescriptive proprioceptive information sufficient for inducing motor learning? PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e37687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wong, J.D.; Kistemaker, D.A.; Chin, A.; Gribble, P.L. Can proprioceptive training improve motor learning? J. Neurophysiol. 2012, 108, 3313–3321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bernardi, N.F.; Darainy, M.; Ostry, D.J. Somatosensory Contribution to the Initial Stages of Human Motor Learning. J. Neurosci. 2015, 35, 14316–14326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sakamoto, T.; Kondo, T. Visuomotor learning by passive motor experience. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bobath, B. Adult Hemiplegia: Evaluation and Treatment London; William Heinnemann: Portsmouth, NH, USA, 1978. [Google Scholar]
- Davis, P.M. Steps to Follow; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Nancie, R.; Fesp, F. Handling the Young Child with Cerebral Palsy at Home; Butterworth-Heinemann: Oxford, UK, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Craske, B.; Crawshaw, M. Shifts in kinesthesis through time and after active and passive movement. Percept. Mot. Skills. 1975, 40, 755–761. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kelso, J.A.S. Planning and efferent components in the coding of movement. J. Mot. Behav. 1977, 9, 33–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Laufer, Y.; Hocherman, S.; Dickstein, R. Accuracy of reproducing hand position when using active compared with passive movement. Physiother. Res. Int. 2001, 6, 65–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Trewartha, K.M.; Case, S.; Flanagan, J.R. Integrating actions into object location memory: A benefit for active versus passive reaching movements. Behav. Brain. Res. 2015, 279, 234–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sawaguchi, Y.; Kawasaki, T.; Hiraoka, K. Effect of Moving Tactile Stimuli to Mimic Altered Weight Distribution During Gait on Quiet Stance Body Sway. Percept. Mot. Skills. 2023, 130, 00315125231197840. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fowler, S.C.; Notterman, J.M. An observed short-term motor memory effect for isometric force emission. Percept. Psychophys. 1975, 17, 393–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hiraoka, K.; Ishimoto, M.; Kishigami, M.; Sakaya, R.; Sumimoto, A.; Yoshikawa, K. Decay of short-term motor memory regarding force reproduction. Motor Control. 2022, 27, 338–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pepper, R.L.; Herman, L.M. Decay and interference effects in the short-term retention of a discrete motor act. J. Exp. Psychol. 1970, 83, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- O’Sullivan, K.; Verschueren, S.; Van Hoof, W.; Ertanir, F.; Martens, L.; Dankaerts, W. Lumbar repositioning error in sitting: Healthy controls versus people with sitting-related non-specific chronic low back pain (flexion pattern). Man. Ther. 2013, 18, 526–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brindle, T.J.; Nitz, A.J.; Uhl, T.L.; Kifer, E.; Shapiro, R. Measures of accuracy for active shoulder movements at 3 different speeds with kinesthetic and visual feedback. J. Orthop. Sports. Phys. Ther. 2004, 34, 468–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Godinho, P.; Nicoliche, E.; Cossich, V.; Sousa, E.B.D.; Velasques, B.; Salles, J.I. Proprioceptive deficit in patients with complete tearing of the anterior cruciate ligament. Rev. Bras. Ortop. 2014, 49, 613–618. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brown, J. Some tests of the decay theory of immediate memory. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1958, 10, 12–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fitts, P.M.; Posner, M.I. Human Performance; Brooks/Cole: Belmont, CA, USA, 1967. [Google Scholar]
- Oda, H.; Sawaguchi, Y.; Kawasaki, T.; Fukuda, S.; Hiraoka, K. Influence of the inter-trial interval, movement observation, and hand dominance on the previous trial effect. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2021, 15, 761514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stone, K.D.; Keizer, A.; Dijkerman, H.C. The influence of vision, touch, and proprioception on body representation of the lower limbs. Acta. Psychol. 2018, 185, 22–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Winter, D.A. Medial-lateral and anterior-posterior motor responses associated with centre of pressure changes in quiet standing. Neurosci. Res. Commun. 1993, 12, 141–148. [Google Scholar]
- Miller, G.A. The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychol. Rev. 1956, 63, 81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cowan, N. The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Brain. Behav. Sci. 2001, 24, 87–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Luck, S.J.; Vogel, E.K. The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature 1997, 390, 279–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ma, W.J.; Husain, M.; Bays, P.M. Changing concepts of working memory. Nat. Neurosci. 2014, 17, 347–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Faust-Adams, A.S. Interference in short-term retention of discrete movements. J. Exp. Psychol. 1972, 96, 400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
L (cm) | R (cm) | F (cm) | B (cm) | L + F (cm) | R + F (cm) | L + B (cm) | R + B (cm) | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
COPx | ||||||||||
1st | Close | 0.0 ± 0.1 | 0.0 ± 0.1 | 0.1 ± 0.1 | 0.0 ± 0.1 | 0.0 ± 0.1 | 0.0 ± 0.1 | −0.1 ± 0.1 | 0.0 ± 0.1 | |
Distant | −0.1 ± 0.1 | 0.0 ± 0.1 | 0.1 ± 0.1 | 0.0 ± 0.1 | −0.1 ± 0.1 | −0.1 ± 0.1 | 0.0 ± 0.1 | 0.0 ± 0.1 | ||
2nd | Close | −0.1 ± 0.1 | 0.2 ± 0.1 | 0.0 ± 0.1 | 0.0 ± 0.1 | 0.0 ± 0.1 | 0.2 ± 0.1 | −0.1 ± 0.2 | 0.0 ± 0.1 | |
Distant | −0.3 ± 0.1 | −0.1 ± 0.1 | −0.1 ± 0.1 | −0.1 ± 0.0 | 0.0 ± 0.1 | 0.0 ± 0.1 | −0.3 ± 0.1 | 0.2 ± 0.1 | ||
COPy | ||||||||||
1st | Close | 0.3 ± 0.1 | 0.2 ± 0.1 | 0.4 ± 0.1 | −0.1 ± 0.1 | 0.3 ± 0.1 * | 0.4 ± 0.1 | −1.1 ± 0.2 * | 0.8 ± 0.1 * | |
Distant | 0.0 ± 0.1 | 0.0 ± 0.1 | 0.1 ± 0.1 | −0.1 ± 0.0 | 0.0 ± 0.1 | 0.2 ± 0.1 | −0.1 ± 0.1 | −0.1 ± 0.1 | ||
2nd | Close | 0.2 ± 0.1 | 0.2 ± 0.1 | 0.3 ± 0.1 | 0.2 ± 0.1 | 0.3 ± 0.1 | 0.4 ± 0.1 | −0.1 ± 0.1 | 0.1 ± 0.1 | |
Distant | −0.3 ± 0.1 | 0.0 ± 0.1 | −0.2 ± 0.1 | 0.0 ± 0.1 | 0.0 ± 0.1 | 0.2 ± 0.1 | −0.4 ± 0.1 | −0.1 ± 0.1 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Oda, H.; Fukuda, S.; Tsujinaka, R.; Gao, H.; Hiraoka, K. Short-Term Reproduction of Active Movement with Visual Feedback and Passive Movement with a Therapist’s Hands. Brain Sci. 2024, 14, 531. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14060531
Oda H, Fukuda S, Tsujinaka R, Gao H, Hiraoka K. Short-Term Reproduction of Active Movement with Visual Feedback and Passive Movement with a Therapist’s Hands. Brain Sciences. 2024; 14(6):531. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14060531
Chicago/Turabian StyleOda, Hitoshi, Shiho Fukuda, Ryo Tsujinaka, Han Gao, and Koichi Hiraoka. 2024. "Short-Term Reproduction of Active Movement with Visual Feedback and Passive Movement with a Therapist’s Hands" Brain Sciences 14, no. 6: 531. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14060531
APA StyleOda, H., Fukuda, S., Tsujinaka, R., Gao, H., & Hiraoka, K. (2024). Short-Term Reproduction of Active Movement with Visual Feedback and Passive Movement with a Therapist’s Hands. Brain Sciences, 14(6), 531. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14060531