[go: up one dir, main page]
More Web Proxy on the site http://driver.im/ Skip to main content
MIT Press Open Journals logoLink to MIT Press Open Journals
. 2021 May 1;33(6):1142–1157. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01702

The Neural Codes Underlying Internally Generated Representations in Visual Working Memory

Qing Yu 1, Bradley R Postle 2
PMCID: PMC8594925  PMID: 34428785

Abstract

Humans can construct rich subjective experience even when no information is available in the external world. Here, we investigated the neural representation of purely internally generated stimulus-like information during visual working memory. Participants performed delayed recall of oriented gratings embedded in noise with varying contrast during fMRI scanning. Their trialwise behavioral responses provided an estimate of their mental representation of the to-be-reported orientation. We used multivariate inverted encoding models to reconstruct the neural representations of orientation in reference to the response. We found that response orientation could be successfully reconstructed from activity in early visual cortex, even on 0% contrast trials when no orientation information was actually presented, suggesting the existence of a purely internally generated neural code in early visual cortex. In addition, cross-generalization and multidimensional scaling analyses demonstrated that information derived from internal sources was represented differently from typical working memory representations, which receive influences from both external and internal sources. Similar results were also observed in intraparietal sulcus, with slightly different cross-generalization patterns. These results suggest a potential mechanism for how externally driven and internally generated information is maintained in working memory.

INTRODUCTION

Humans have the ability to mentally retain and manipulate visual information even when the information is not in view. This ability—visual working memory—is fundamental to human cognition (Luck & Vogel, 2013; Baddeley, 2003; Engle, Tuholski, Laughlin, & Conway, 1999). Understanding how the brain keeps such information online is thus a critical question for cognitive neuroscience. The sensorimotor recruitment hypothesis posits that sensory cortex is an important substrate for the representation of fine-grained perceptual information in working memory (Serences, 2016; D'Esposito & Postle, 2015; Awh & Jonides, 2001), for example, early visual cortex for maintaining low-level visual information. This view is supported by evidence from multivariate analyses of fMRI data that stimulus-specific information can be decoded from early visual cortex during maintenance of visual feature information (Yu & Shim, 2017; Riggall & Postle, 2012; Harrison & Tong, 2009; Serences, Ester, Vogel, & Awh, 2009). With fMRI, a neural code is assessed as a systematic set of mappings between different values of a cognitive state and different patterns of fMRI activity, and a shared code is inferred if the same mapping is observed across two domains of cognition. With this logic, it has been demonstrated that, in early visual cortex, visual working memory shares the same neural codes with visual perception (Harrison & Tong, 2009), attention (Yu & Shim, 2019), and imagery (Albers, Kok, Toni, Dijkerman, & de Lange, 2013), suggesting that early visual cortex may serve as a mental buffer for representing visual information across different categories of cognitive task (Roelfsema & de Lange, 2016).

Although early visual cortex recruits common neural codes for different cognitive processes, these processes can be driven by distinct sources of information. For example, visual perception is driven by external, bottom–up input received from the retina, and visual imagery is driven by internal, top–down input from higher cortical areas (Pearson, 2019). Of course, early visual cortex contains numerous reciprocal connections with higher cortical areas, and bottom–up and top–down signaling are involved in most, if not all, visually mediated behaviors, including visual perception (Gilbert & Li, 2013; Muckli & Petro, 2013). Nevertheless, the fact that visual imagery shows distinct temporal dynamics and evolves later in time, compared with visual perception (Dijkstra, Mostert, de Lange, Bosch, & van Gerven, 2018), suggests at least some meaningful distinction between the processing of externally presented and internally generated information.

When considering the sensorimotor recruitment hypothesis, it is important to note that visual working memory cannot be understood as merely the prolongation of sensory processing, because many stimulus-nonspecific factors can influence representations in working memory. For example, several studies have demonstrated recall biases toward discrete color centers in visual working memory for color (Panichello, DePasquale, Pillow, & Buschman, 2019; Bae, Olkkonen, Allred, & Flombaum, 2015; Bae, Olkkonen, Allred, Wilson, & Flombaum, 2014), probably because of drift toward stable attractor states established through prior experience (Panichello et al., 2019). Information from the previous trial can also be reactivated or otherwise influence the current trial (Barbosa et al., 2020; Bae & Luck, 2019). Moreover, there is considerable physiological evidence for an important role for feedback from higher cortical areas. For example, laminar recordings indicate that delay-period input to primary visual cortex (V1) is most prominent in supra- and infragranular layers that receive feedback projections from higher areas (Lawrence et al., 2018; van Kerkoerle, Self, & Roelfsema, 2017), and delay-period local field potentials in the motion-sensitive middle temporal area MT are coherent with spiking in PFC (Mendoza-Halliday, Torres, & Martinez-Trujillo, 2014). Because typical working memory tasks, including those cited here, begin with the external presentation of to-be-remembered stimulus information, delay-period representations presumably reflect the combined influence of processes associated with externally presented and with internally generated information. How such “typical” working memory representations may differ from purely internally generated representations is the focus of this study. Although our focus is on representations in early visual cortex, we also present results from intraparietal sulcus (IPS), because this region has also been implicated in representing working memory-related information (e.g., Yu & Shim, 2017; Bettencourt & Xu, 2016; Ester, Sprague, & Serences, 2015).

In the current study, stimulus contrast varied across trials between 0%, 10%, and 60%, but participants were instructed that a sample orientation would be presented on every trial and that a recall response was required at the end of every trial, regardless of subjective visibility. This allowed us to use responses to infer backward what they had represented during the delay period, including on 0% contrast (“null”) trials that lacked external input. These responses could then be used to investigate internally generated representations in visual working memory maintenance. The comparison between the null and typical working memory trials (with 10% or 60% contrast) could also be used to isolate processes specific to internally generated representations.

METHODS

Participants

All participants were recruited from the University of Wisconsin–Madison community. Two behavioral experiments (Experiments 1A and 1B) were performed prior to the fMRI experiment (Experiment 2) to test the visibility of the stimuli to be used in the fMRI experiment. Thirteen individuals (2 men, mean age = 21.0 ± 3.3 years) participated in Experiment 1A, and 7 of these also participated in Experiment 1B, along with 9 new individuals (n = 16 in total; 3 men, mean age = 19.6 ± 1.9 years). Eighteen individuals (including one who also participated in Experiment 1B) participated in Experiment 2. One of these was excluded because of failure to comply with task instructions, resulting in 17 individuals (4 men, mean age = 23.5 ± 3.8 years) as the final sample size for Experiment 2. We did not carry out power analysis a priori, but our sample size was comparable or even superior to those from recent fMRI studies that have used a similar task design (Yu, Teng, & Postle, 2020; Rademaker, Chunharas, & Serences, 2019; Bettencourt & Xu, 2016; Ester et al., 2015). All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, reported no neurological or psychiatric disease, and provided written informed consent approved by the University of Wisconsin–Madison Health Sciences institutional review board. All were monetarily compensated for their participation.

Stimuli and Procedure

Sample stimuli were sinusoidal gratings embedded in white noise (spatial frequency = 1°/cycle, radius = 4°), presented at varying levels of Michelson contrast. In Experiment 1A, there were two types of stimuli: gratings with a high contrast (60%) and gratings with a 75% threshold-level contrast, determined for each participant with a thresholding task. In Experiments 1B and 2, there were three types of stimuli: gratings with a high contrast (contrast = 60%), gratings with a low contrast (contrast = 10%), and null stimuli (contrast = 0%). Importantly, no orientation information was visible in null gratings, making them equivalent to white noise patches.

All stimuli were created and presented using MATLAB (The MathWorks) and Psychtoolbox 3 extensions (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997). In Experiments 1A and 1B, stimuli were presented on a 21.5-in. iMac screen at a viewing distance of 63 cm and behavioral responses were made with a computer mouse. In Experiment 2, stimuli were projected via a 60-Hz projector (Avotec Silent Vision 6011, Avotec, Inc.) and viewed through a coil-mounted mirror in the MRI scanner at a viewing distance of 69 cm, and participants' behavioral responses were made with an MR-compatible trackball response pad (Current Designs Inc.). During the scan, eye position was monitored and recorded using the Avotec RE-5700 eye-tracking system (Avotec, Inc.).

Experiment 2

We begin with a detailed description of Experiment 2, the experiment of primary theoretical interest, during which participants performed one-item delayed recall of orientation in the fMRI scanner. On each trial, participants viewed a sample stimulus (high, low, or null) presented at the center of the screen for 0.5 sec. After a delay of 9.5 sec (or 8.5 sec for two participants), an orientation dial (radius = 4°) was presented centrally, and participants rotated the dial until its needle matched the remembered orientation as precisely as possible in a 4-sec response window. Critically, participants were told that an oriented grating would be presented on every trial, although its visibility would vary across trials, and they were instructed to make a best guess when they were unsure about what the orientation was. Feedback (recall error) was provided after the response period for 0.5 sec, even on null trials, and recall error was calculated as the angular difference between sample and response orientations, regardless of whether or not the sample orientation had actually been visible (Figure 1). The sample orientation for each trial was randomly selected from 1° to 180° in steps of 1° in the orientation space. The starting position of the needle of the response dial was randomly chosen on every trial, independent of the sample.

Figure 1. .


Figure 1. 

Trial sequence of the fMRI task. Participants performed a one-item delayed recall task on oriented gratings. On different trials, they viewed a high-contrast (60%) grating embedded in noise, a low-contrast (10%) grating embedded in noise, or a null-contrast (0%) grating embedded in noise (i.e., pure noise patch). Participants were told orientation information was always presented, despite that the visibility of the gratings might differ. After a prolonged delay (8.5 sec for two participants and 9.5 sec for 15 participants), they recalled the remembered orientation on an orientation wheel. Feedback was provided at the end of every response.

For four participants, total trial length was 22 sec: For two of the four participants tested with an 8.5-sec delay (S01 and S02), the intertrial interval (ITI) was 9 sec, and for the two tested with a 9.5-sec delay (S03 and S04), ITI was 8 sec. For all remaining participants, for whom the delay was 9.5 sec and ITI was 10 sec, total trial length was 24 sec. To match the number of time points across participants, all analyses focused on the first 22 sec of every trial.

Each run began with an 8-sec fixation period, followed by 18 experimental trials, and the ratio of trial types (high:low:null) during each run was 3:1:2 (i.e., nine high trials, three low trials, and six null trials). For one participant (S01), the experimental run in the first scan session was truncated to 12 trials (i.e., six high trials, two low trials, four null trials) because of a technical problem with scanning. Each participant completed 28–32 runs across two scanning sessions. In total, 12 participants completed 270 high trials, 90 low trials, and 180 null trials (S02, S05, S06, S08 to S12, S14 to S17); two participants completed 288 high trials, 96 low trials, and 192 null trials (S03 and S04); two completed 252 high trials, 84 low trials, and 168 null trials (S07 and S13); and one (S01) completed 231 high trials, 77 low trials, and 154 null trials. All participants were debriefed at the end of the study, and none of them reported awareness of the existence of null trials (i.e., all reported believing that an oriented grating was presented on every trial).

Experiments 1A and 1B

Prior to the fMRI experiment, we ran two behavioral studies to determine the contrasts of the gratings to be used in the scanner. The overarching rationale was to develop conditions that would disguise from participants the fact that a substantial proportion of samples contained no stimulus information (i.e., null samples). To achieve this, we sought to find two levels of contrast that were each highly discriminable, but that would create the impression for participants that subjective visibility would vary from trial to trial. The trial structure for both was similar to that from Experiment 2: one sample grating (radius = 3°) with a randomly selected orientation was presented on the screen for 0.1 sec, followed by a brief delay, followed by recall with an orientation wheel. Responses were self-paced, and feedback was given after each response (0.5 sec).

Experiment 1A was carried out to examine how participants would perform at each of two levels of contrast: high and at-threshold. It began with a block of 80 trials to determine each individual's contrast threshold: After an initial 10 trials of delayed recall (delay of 0.3 sec) at a fixed contrast of 12%, the sample contrast for each of the ensuing trials was adjusted using a QUEST procedure (Watson & Pelli, 1983). Responses were binarized using a cutoff criterion of 20° of recall error. Four catch trials were interleaved at randomly determined intervals, and on these catch trials, the contrast was set to 3 times of the contrast from QUEST. The discrimination contrast threshold of the grating that generated 75% accuracy was determined at the end of the block. During the remainder of the session, participants performed five or six blocks of delayed recall of orientation, delay length was either 1 sec or 7 sec, and delay length and sample contrast (60%; at threshold) were fully crossed during each 60-trial block.

Experiment 1B was carried out to examine how participants would perform at each of the three levels of contrast that would be used for the fMRI study: high (60%), low (10%), and null (0%). Participants performed five or six blocks with 60 trials each; again, delay length was either 1 or 7 sec, and delay length and sample contrast (high; low; null) were fully crossed. For both Experiments 1A and 1B, only trials with a 7-sec delay were included in the behavioral analyses to better match the duration of the fMRI task.

Behavioral Analyses

Behavioral performance was assessed in two ways. Within-trial recall error was calculated for high and low trials as the angular difference between the sample and response orientations, for each condition separately. Differences between conditions were evaluated by paired t tests. Serial bias on response from the previous trial was calculated for all three conditions. This was done by calculating the difference between the current and previous response, and grouping the difference values into nine 20°-wide bins. To test whether the number of trials differed between bins, we performed a χ2 goodness of fit test on each condition.

fMRI Methods

Data Acquisition

Whole-brain images were acquired with a 3 Tesla GE MRI scanner (Discovery MR750; GE Healthcare) with a 32-channel head coil at the Lane Neuroimaging Laboratory at the University of Wisconsin–Madison HealthEmotions Research Institute (Department of Psychiatry). Functional images were acquired with a gradient-echo echo-planar sequence (2 sec repetition time (TR), 22-msec echo time, 60° flip angle) within a 64 × 64 matrix (42 axial slices, 3 mm isotropic). A high-resolution T1 image was also acquired for each session with a fast SPGR echo sequence (8.2-msec TR, 3.2-msec echo time, 12° flip angle, 176 axial slices, 256 × 256 in-plane, 1.0 mm isotropic).

Preprocessing

fMRI data were preprocessed using AFNI (afni.nimh.nih.gov; Cox, 1996). The first four volumes of each functional run were removed. The data were then registered to the first volume of the first run within each scan session and then to the T1 volume of the same session. Data from the second session were further registered to the T1 volume of the first scanning session. The data were then motion corrected, detrended (linear, quadratic, cubic), and converted to percent signal change. Data for subsequent general linear model analyses were further spatially smoothed with a 4-mm FWHM Gaussian kernel. Data for multivariate and univariate time course analyses were z-scored within each run.

Univariate Analyses

Task-related changes in activity were identified with a mass-univariate general linear model implemented in AFNI, with sample, delay, and probe epochs of the task modeled with boxcars (0.5, 8.5 or 9.5 sec depending on the participant, and 4 sec, respectively), each convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function. Six nuisance regressors were also included to account for head motion artifacts in the six dimensions of rigid body motion.

Percent signal change in BOLD activity relative to baseline was calculated for each time point during the working memory task; baseline was chosen as the average BOLD activity of the first TR of each trial. BOLD signal change was averaged across trials within each condition and across all voxels within each ROI (see below).

Statistical significance of BOLD activity against baseline was assessed using two-tailed, one-sample t tests against 0, and the resultant p values were corrected across time points and comparisons using FDR (false discovery rate; Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995). Statistical difference of BOLD activity between conditions at each time point was assessed using two-tailed paired t tests, with FDR correction applied across time points and comparisons.

ROI Definition

We created subject-specific anatomical ROIs by warping masks from the probabilistic atlas of Wang, Mruczek, Arcaro, and Kastner (2015) to each participant's structural scan in their native space. Early visual anatomical ROIs were created by merging the masks for unilateral V1, V2, and V3 within and between hemispheres. IPS anatomical ROIs were created by merging the masks for unilateral regions IPS0-5 within and between hemispheres. For the Early Visual Cortex functionally defined ROI, we identified the 500 voxels displaying the strongest loading on the contrast (sample − baseline), collapsing over all three conditions. For the IPS functionally defined ROI, we identified the 500 voxels displaying the strongest loading on the contrast (delay − baseline), collapsing over all three conditions. For completeness, an alternate “Sample IPS ROI” was also defined as the 500 voxels in this anatomical region displaying the strongest loading on the contrast (sample − baseline).

Multivariate Inverted Encoding Modeling

All inverted encoding modeling (IEM) analyses were performed using custom functions in MATLAB. The IEM assumes that the responses of each voxel can be characterized by a small number of hypothesized tuning channels. After previous work, the number of orientation tuning channels was set to nine (20° apart, equally spaced), and the idealized feature tuning curve of each channel to a specific orientation θ was defined as a half-wave-rectified sinusoid raised to the eighth power (FWHM = 0.82 rad):

fθ=cosθc8

where c was the center of the channel.

We then computed the weight matrix (W, v × k, v: the number of voxels; k: the number of channels) that projects the hypothesized channel responses (C1, k × n, n: the number of trials) to actual measured fMRI signals in the training dataset (B1, v × n), and extracted the estimated channel responses (Ĉ2, k × n) for the test data set (B2, v × n) using this weight matrix.

The relationship between the training data set (B1) and the channel responses (C1) was characterized by:

B1=WC1

Therefore, the least-squared estimate of the weight matrix (Ŵ) was calculated using linear regression:

Wˆ=B1C1TC1C1T1

The channel responses (Ĉ2) for the test data set (B2) was then estimated using the weight matrix (Ŵ):

Cˆ2=WˆTWˆ1WˆTB2

Because orientations in the current study were randomly selected from the 1–180° orientation space (in steps of 1°), we did not pick a fixed set of channel centers, as is often done (Yu, Teng, et al., 2020; Yu & Shim, 2017). Instead, after Rademaker et al. (2019) we first picked a set of equally spaced channel centers (e.g., 0°, 20°, 40°, 60°, 80°, 100°, 120°, 140°, 160°), conducted the analysis as described above, and then shifted the channel centers by 1° and repeated the analysis. The procedure was repeated 20 times, such that all 180 orientations from 1° to 180° in 1° step served as channel centers. We then combined estimated channel responses from all iterations of these analyses to create responses of 180 orientation channels. The result, for any given orientation, can be considered a reconstruction of the model's estimate of the neural representation of that orientation. This procedure ensured that our channel estimates were not biased by any specific channel centers. All channel responses were then centered on a common center (0° on the x axis) and averaged for visualization and for statistical comparisons.

Hypothesis Testing

Analysis plan.

If a participant is not aware of the fact that a considerable proportion of trials will feature null samples that contain no orientation information, we assume that, on null trials, they will generate an orientation for response at some point prior to the onset of the response dial. Furthermore, because the initial orientation of the dial cannot be predicted prior to its onset, we assume that this response plan will not be kinematic (e.g., how many degrees they plan to turn the dial), but rather will be the representation of the orientation that the participant plans to produce at the end of trial. To validate this assumption, our first analysis would be to train an IEM using the orientation of the response on that trial (response-based IEM). Successful reconstruction of orientation with this IEM at time points preceding the response (i.e., during the delay period) would mean that participants were indeed representing the orientation of their planned response during those earlier time points (response-based neural code).

Assuming success of this first analysis, the next step would be to determine whether a common response-based neural code was employed across conditions. This would be done by applying the response-based IEM from one trial type (e.g., high) to data from the other two trial types (e.g., low and null). We anticipated three possible outcomes: If reconstruction in a tested condition was significantly positive, and did not differ from that in the training condition, this would reflect “full generalization”; if reconstruction in a tested condition was significantly positive, but was also significantly lower than that in the training condition, this would reflect “partial generalization”; and if reconstruction in a tested condition was not significant, this would reflect “failed generalization.” These results would be interpreted as evidence for a fully shared neural code, for a partly shared neural code, or as a failure to find evidence for a shared neural code, respectively.

Finally, because IEM relies on specific hypotheses of orientation channels, we would also perform a model-free analysis, multidimensional scaling (MDS), to see if this alternative approach would support conclusions comparable to those suggested by the IEM analyses.

Operationalizing hypothesis tests.

To investigate the codes supporting the representation of orientation in the different conditions (high; low; null), we trained two IEMs: a response-based IEM labeled according to the orientation of the response on each trial and a sample-based IEM labeled according to the sample orientation on each trial. Note that the response-based IEM would be the focus of our analyses, and results from the sample-based IEM in the null condition would not be interpretable on their own, but would serve as controls for comparing with the results from high and low conditions. IEMs were trained and tested using a leave-one-run-out cross-validation procedure, for each condition, time point (or average of time points, e.g., average of time points 8–10 sec for delay period), and ROI separately. To compare response-based neural codes across conditions, we also used a leave-one-run-out procedure, training the response-based IEM on data from one condition, and testing the IEM on the data from all three conditions, including the training condition (which would yield the same result as the first analysis) and the two other conditions. This procedure was performed for each condition, time point (or average of time points), and ROI separately.

We also trained several complementary IEMs for testing alternative explanations for the results. First, we trained a mixed IEM using a balanced set of trials from each condition (high; low; null) and tested this IEM on the same balanced set of trials from each condition separately. The purpose of this IEM would be to avoid potential concerns with differences in signal-to-noise ratio across IEMs (Liu, Cable, & Gardner, 2018; Sprague et al., 2018). Second, to examine the influence of previous-trial information on the reconstruction of current-trial information, we trained response-based IEMs using response labels from the previous trial, or trained sample-based and response-based IEMs while excluding trials with similar response to that of the previous trial.

To characterize the strength of each IEM reconstruction, we collapsed over the channel responses on both sides of the common center, averaged them, then calculated the slope of each collapsed reconstruction using linear regression (Foster, Bsales, Jaffe, & Awh, 2017; Samaha, Sprague, & Postle, 2016). A larger positive slope indicates stronger positive representation. We used a bootstrapping procedure (Yu, Teng, et al., 2020; Ester et al., 2015) to characterize the significance of the slopes. For each combination of factors (IEM, condition, time point, or ROI), 17 orientation reconstructions were randomly sampled with replacement from the pool of 17 participants and averaged. This procedure was repeated 10,000 times, resulting in 10,000 average orientation reconstructions, and correspondingly 10,000 slopes. The probability of obtaining a negative slope among the 10,000 slopes was counted as the one-tailed p value of the slope. To characterize the difference between the slopes of two IEM reconstructions, we first calculated the difference between two bootstrapped slopes 10,000 times, which generated 10,000 slope differences. The significance of the slope difference was then calculated using the same one-tailed method as above. All p values were corrected for multiple comparisons using the FDR method, across IEMs (sample-based, response-based), conditions (high, low, or null), and time points.

We also assessed evidence for differences between the slopes of delay-period response-based reconstructions with Bayes Factors (BF), which support evaluation of the amount of evidence for one hypothesis (H1) against the null hypothesis (H0). H1 referred to a positive reconstruction, and H0 referred to a failed reconstruction (i.e., a slope no larger than 0). For comparison between the slopes of two reconstructions, H1 referred to the slopes being different and H0 referred to the absence of evidence for a difference. As an example, a BF10 of 3 would indicate that H1 is 3 times more likely than H0, whereas a BF10 of 0.33 would indicate that H0 is 3 times more likely than H1. All BF analyses were conducted using JASP (Love et al., 2019).

MDS.

For each ROI and each trial epoch (sample: 4–6 sec after trial onset, delay: 8–10 sec after trial onset, and response: 14–16 sec after trial onset), we categorized all response orientations into four bins (0–45°, 45–90°, 90–135°, and 135–180°). Trial number for each condition was matched by subsampling data from the high and null conditions to match the number of trials in the low condition. The Euclidean distances between orientation bins and conditions were then computed using the covariance matrix calculated from the subsampled data. This subsampling procedure was repeated for 1000 times and averaged. Distances were averaged across participants, and MDS was performed on the distance matrix using the cmdscale function in MATLAB.

RESULTS

Behavior

Experiment 1A

Participants' 75% contrast discrimination threshold for recall of orientation against a noise background ranged between 4% and 6%, with a mean of 5.0% and a standard deviation of 0.6%. For delayed recall of the orientation of a sample grating, the average recall error for high contrast (60%) samples (9.0° ± 1.5°) was significantly lower than for the threshold contrast samples (17.4° ± 4.4°), t(12) = 5.95, p < .001.

Experiment 1B

Delayed recall of orientation did not differ between high contrast (60%; 10.2° ± 1.9°) and low contrast (10%; 10.6° ± 2.7°) conditions, t(15) = 0.64, p = .530 (Figure 1). The fact that average recall error did not differ between the high and low trials established the fact, critical for the logic of Experiment 2, that low and high samples were comparably visible to participants. This, plus the marked difference between performance at these two levels of contrast versus performance at 75% threshold (Experiment 1A), indicated that neither high nor low contrast trials were likely to produce trials in which the sample grating was not visible to the participant (in contrast to some of the threshold trials in Experiment 1A).

Experiment 2

Consistent with Experiment 1B, recall error during scanning did not differ between the high (11.4° ± 4.0°) and low (11.7° ± 4.8°) trials, t(16) = 0.58, p = .567.

Although recall error could not be calculated for null trials, the results from several analyses suggested that participants did not treat null trials different from trials on which a sample grating was visible. First, angular difference between the starting position of the response needle and the recalled orientation did not differ between high, low, and null trials (42.3° ± 2.4°, 41.7° ± 3.9°, 41.8° ± 5.7°, respectively; all ts < 0.85, ps > .408), suggesting that the three conditions were comparable in terms of effort during recall. Second, although sample orientation on each trial was randomly chosen and the distribution of sample orientations was uniform (i.e., there was an equal proportion of cardinal and oblique orientations), plotting the distribution of participants' raw responses showed biased responses toward oblique orientations (relative to cardinal orientations) for all three trial types (Figure 2). This indicates that trials of all types were influenced to a similar extent by a systematic bias, perhaps from one or more stimulus-nonspecific factors such as prior knowledge (Yu, Panichello, Cai, Postle, & Buschman, 2020; Panichello et al., 2019). In summary, null and high/low trials were well matched in terms of procedural details, and the only difference between conditions was the availability of external orientation information. Therefore, any orientation information observed in the null trials could only be internally generated.

Figure 2. .


Figure 2. 

Raw response distribution. The raw response distribution of high, low, and null conditions, indicated by the gray histograms. The black lines indicate the envelope of sample distribution.

fMRI

Time Course of BOLD Activity

All analyses were carried out at the level of the Early Visual Cortex ROI and the IPS ROI. In both regions, a conventional time course of BOLD activity change was observed for all three conditions (Figure 3): Sample-evoked activity reached its peak at around 4–6 sec after trial onset; delay-period activity reached its trough at around 8–10 sec; and response-evoked activity reached its peak at around 14–16 sec. Time points 8–10 sec were subsequently used to operationalize “late delay-period” activity. In early visual cortex, activity in null trials was slightly lower than that in high and low trials during sample and early delay epochs (2–8 sec; all ps < .023), but not at 10 sec (both ps > .167) nor during the response epoch (12 sec and after; all ps > .342). In IPS, in contrast, null activity was lower during the sample (2–4 sec; all ps < .005) and response epochs (12–18 sec, all ps < .040 except for 12 sec between high and null: p = .073), but not during the delay (6–10 sec, all ps > .132).

Figure 3. .


Figure 3. 

Time course of BOLD activity in early visual cortex and IPS. (A) Trial-averaged BOLD activity in the Early Visual Cortex ROI. (B) Time course of BOLD activity in the IPS ROI. Dark blue, light blue, and gray lines correspond to the high, low, and null conditions, respectively. Data from the two participants with 8.5-sec delay periods were included in the averaged results, but event labels below the x axis represent the trial sequence for participants with 9.5-sec delay periods only for illustration purposes in this and subsequent figures, with S, D, R representing the Sample, Delay, and Response periods, respectively. Shaded areas indicate ± 1 SEM. *p < .05

IEM

Early visual cortex.

To assess the time course of neural representations of orientation, for each trial type, we applied a sample-based IEM (i.e., trained on the sample label) and a response-based IEM (trained on the response label) to every time point of the trial. For high and low trials, reconstruction with the sample-based IEM was significant beginning at 4 sec after sample onset and sustained for the remainder of the trial (all ps < .001). Similarly, reconstruction with the response-based IEM were significant for the duration of trial, beginning at 4 sec for high trials and at 2 sec for low trials (all ps < .040). Sample-based and response-based reconstructions did not differ at any time point, for either of these two conditions (all ps > .157). These results validated the approach of using participants' responses as an estimate of the orientation that they represented earlier in the trial, prior to the response.

Turning next to null trials, reconstruction with sample-based IEMs did not achieve statistical significance except for two isolated time points: 2 sec (p = .017) and 16 sec (p = .036), probably because of statistical noise. Note that these null results amounted to confirmation of a sanity check, because the labels used to train the sample-based IEM did not correspond to what participants were presented on these trials. Reconstructions with response-based IEMs were significant beginning with 6 sec and for the duration of the trial (all ps < .020; Figure 4A). Critically, these response-based reconstructions were significantly different from the sample-based reconstructions for 6–8 sec and from 12 sec onward (green asterisks; all ps < .012), suggesting that robust orientation representations specific to the response started from 6 sec after trial onset. This indicates that, beginning relatively early in the trial, participants generated and maintained a representation with exclusively internally derived information.

Figure 4. .


Figure 4. 

Orientation reconstructions in early visual cortex and IPS in the high, low, and null conditions. (A) Time course of orientation reconstructions (indexed by the slope of reconstruction) in early visual cortex, from left to right: in high, low, and null condition. (B) Orientation reconstruction in the late delay period (8–10 sec after trial onset) in early visual cortex. (C) Time course of orientation reconstructions (indexed by the slope of reconstruction) in IPS, from left to right: in high, low, and null condition. (D) Orientation reconstruction in the late delay period in IPS. Black and red lines correspond to sample and response, respectively. Black and red asterisks at the top of each plot denote significance of reconstructions, for sample and response, respectively. Green asterisks denote significance of difference between response and sample reconstructions. Event labels below the x axis represent the Sample (S), Delay (D), and Response (R) periods, respectively. All p values were corrected with FDR across conditions and time points. Shaded areas denote ± 1 SEM. *p < .05.

IPS.

In IPS, results were generally comparable to those from early visual cortex, albeit weaker in magnitude. When focusing on the late delay period (Figure 4D), sample and response reconstructions were significant in all conditions (all ps < .037), except for the sample reconstruction in the null condition (p = .259). Time-point-by-time-point reconstructions were also qualitatively similar to early visual cortex (Figure 4C): on high trials sample and response reconstructions emerged during the sample period and were sustained throughout the trial, as were sample reconstructions on low trials (all ps < .041). Response reconstructions were smaller in slope on low trials and, with the exception of a single time point (6 sec after trial onset, p = .007), did not survive correction for multiple comparisons during the delay. Note that the lower number of trials for the low condition might have been responsible for the lack of significance here. Indeed, robust reconstruction of orientation was observed for low trials when averaging across time points in the delay period (Figure 4D).

Turning to null trials, reconstructions with sample-based IEMs only achieved statistical significance at 2 sec, a result probably because of statistical noise. Reconstruction with response-trained IEMs, however, was significant for all time points beginning with 4 sec (all ps < .028), with the exception of 10 sec of the delay period (p = .076).

We also carried out these analyses in the Sample IPS ROI (IPS ROI defined using sample-period activity), and the results (not shown) were qualitatively similar to those in the Delay IPS ROI.

One possible concern about the finding of principal theoretical interest from these analyses—the reconstruction of response-related stimulus information from the delay period of null trials (Figure 4)—is that this might reflect “spillover” of information processed during the previous trial, rather than evidence for genuinely internally generated stimulus representations. Additional analyses carried out to assess this alternative possibility ruled it out as a major concern, and these are presented at the end of the Results section (see Secondary Analyses to Assess the Influence of the Previous Trial on Response-based IEMs section).

Comparison of Neural Codes across High, Low, and Null Trials

Having established robust measurements for internally generated neural representations of orientation, we next sought to examine the nature of these representations. Specifically, because representations on null trials were purely internally generated, whereas representations on high and low trials reflected influences from both external and internal sources, we tested whether the representations maintained during these different trial types recruited a common neural code, in keeping with previous demonstrations of a shared neural code between working memory and perception (Harrison & Tong, 2009), between working memory and attention (Yu & Shim, 2019), and between working memory and imagery (Albers et al., 2013). To this end, we trained IEMs on one condition and tested them on all three conditions (see Methods section). Note that only response-based IEMs were recruited for this purpose. For these analyses, we emphasized the results from the late delay period (8–10 sec after trial onset; also see Figure 5 for results for the full time courses). Here, we also employed BF to assess the amount of evidence in generalization. A BF of larger than 3 or smaller than 1/3 can be considered substantial evidence supporting or rejecting the hypothesis.

Figure 5. .


Figure 5. 

Time course of generalization of neural codes between the high, low, and null conditions. (A) Time course of orientation reconstructions for high, low, and null conditions in early visual cortex, from left to right: results from high-, low-, and null-trained IEMs. (B) Time course of orientation reconstructions for high, low, and null conditions in IPS, from left to right: results from high-, low-, and null-trained IEMs. Dark blue, light blue, and gray lines correspond to the high, low, and null conditions, respectively. Dark blue, light blue, and gray asterisks at the top of each plot denote significance of reconstruction at each time point relative to baseline, for high, low, and null conditions, respectively. Event labels below the x axis represent the Sample (S), Delay (D), and Response (R) periods, respectively. All p values were corrected with FDR across conditions and time points. Shaded areas indicate ± 1 SEM. *p < .05.

In early visual cortex, we successfully reconstructed orientation from the late delay period of low trials with IEMs trained on high trials (p < .001, BF10 = 280.3), and of high trials with IEMs trained on low trials (p < .001, BF10 = 79.5). Furthermore, these results demonstrated full generalization: Reconstructions for high and low trials with the high-trained IEM did not differ from each other (p = .552, BF10 = 0.5); nor did reconstructions for high and low trials with the low-trained IEM (p = .477, BF10 = 0.8; Figure 6A and 6B). When comparing each of these visible trial types with null trials, in contrast, cross-condition generalization was asymmetric: For high trials, although the IEM trained on high trials failed to generalize to null trials (p = .135, BF10 = 0.7; Figure 6A), the IEM trained on null trials did successfully reconstruct orientation on high trials (p = .0014, BF10 = 6.0), and reconstructions for high and null trials with the null-trained IEM did not differ from each other (p = .552, BF10 = 0.2; Figure 6C). For low trials, on one hand, the IEM trained on null trials successfully reconstructed orientation on low trials (p = .0013, BF10 = 8.8), and reconstructions for low and null trials with the null-trained IEM did not differ from each other (p = .552, BF10 = 0.2; Figure 6C); on the other hand, the IEM trained on low trials did generalize to null trials (p = .0052, BF10 = 5.3), although the slope of this reconstruction was lower than that on low trials with the low-trained IEM (p = .030, BF10 = 28.4; Figure 6B), suggesting only partial generalization from low to null trials.

Figure 6. .


Figure 6. 

Generalization of neural codes between the high, low, and null conditions. (A) Left panel: orientation reconstructions for high, low, and null conditions from the high-trained IEM, in the late delay period (8–10 sec after trial onset) in early visual cortex. Right panel: slopes of these reconstructions. (B). Orientation reconstructions and slopes for high, low, and null conditions from the low-trained IEM, in the late delay period (8–10 sec after trial onset) in early visual cortex. (C) Orientation reconstructions and slopes for high, low, and null conditions from the null-trained IEM, in the late delay period (8–10 sec after trial onset) in early visual cortex. (D) Orientation reconstructions and slopes for high, low, and null conditions from the high-trained IEM, in the late delay period (8–10 sec after trial onset) in IPS. (E) Orientation reconstructions and slopes for high, low, and null conditions from the low-trained IEM, in the late delay period (8–10 sec after trial onset) in IPS. (F) Orientation reconstructions and slopes for high, low, and null conditions from the null-trained IEM, in the late delay period (8–10 sec after trial onset) in IPS. Dark blue, light blue, and gray colors correspond to the high, low, and null conditions, respectively. Dark blue, light blue, and gray asterisks at the top of each plot denote significance of reconstruction, for high, low, and null conditions, respectively. All p values were corrected with FDR across conditions. Shaded areas and error bars indicate ± 1 SEM. *p < .05.

In IPS, although response-based neural codes were also fully generalizable between high and low trials (train high–test low, p = .007, BF10 = 6.2; train low–test high, p = .006, BF10 = 6.9; train high–test high vs. train high–test low, p = .732, BF10 = 0.2; train low–test low vs. train low–test high, p = .497, BF10 = 0.3; Figure 6D and 6E), there was no evidence for cross-generalization from null trials to high or low trials (train null–test high, p = .215, BF10 = 0.5; train null–test low, p = .061, BF10 = 1.6; Figure 6F), nor from high or low trials to null trials (train high–test null, p = .252, BF10 = 0.4; and train low–test null, p = .187, BF10 = .6; Figure 6D and 6E).

Although cross-generalization is a common approach for assessing commonality of neural codes (Rademaker et al., 2019; Yu & Shim, 2019; Albers et al., 2013), interpreting failures to generalize can be complicated by technical considerations arising from training the IEM on the same versus on different data sets (Liu et al., 2018; Sprague et al., 2018). Therefore, we repeated these analyses but with a single IEM trained on a balanced set of trials drawn in equal number from high, low, and null trials. Results with this mixed IEM were complementary to the cross-generalization analyses: In both early visual cortex and IPS, the mixed IEM generated successful reconstructions of orientation from high and low trials (4–10 sec: all ps < .006), but failed on null trials (4–10 sec: all ps > .140; Figure 7).

Figure 7. .


Figure 7. 

Time course of generalization of neural codes between the high, low, and null conditions, using a mixed IEM. (A) Time course, in early visual cortex, of orientation reconstructions for high, low, and null conditions using a mixed IEM. (B) Time course, in IPS, of orientation reconstructions for high, low, and null conditions using a mixed IEM. Dark blue, light blue, and gray lines correspond to the high, low, and null conditions, respectively. Dark blue, light blue, and gray asterisks at the top of each plot denote significance of reconstruction at each time point relative to baseline, for high, low, and null conditions, respectively. Event labels below the x axis represent the Sample (S), Delay (D), and Response (R) periods, respectively. All p values were corrected with FDR across conditions and time points. Shaded areas indicate ± 1 SEM. *p < .05.

Model-free Analyses

Lastly, to determine whether a difference between null and high/low trials would be observed when no model-based approach was applied to the data, we compared the representational distances between conditions using MDS. MDS analyses were performed for the sample (4–6 sec after trial onset), delay (8–10 sec after trial onset), and response (14–16 sec after trial onset) epochs of the working memory task, separately for early visual cortex and for IPS. For visualization purposes, response orientations were grouped into four 45°-wide bins.

In early visual cortex, during the sample epoch, the three conditions were discriminable along Dimension 1, confirming that differences in stimulus contrast influenced sensory processing (Figure 8A). During the delay period, the distance between the high and low conditions decreased, such that the two now overlapped along Dimension 1, whereas the null condition remained separated from the other two. This suggested that, as stimulus-driven influences diminished, trials that relied exclusively on internally derived information remained distinct. This discriminative element carried on into the response period, along Dimensions 2 and 3, despite the fact that participants performed the same type of motor response on every trial. In IPS, a similar discriminative pattern was also observed between conditions (Figure 8B). Thus, in both brain areas, patterns of activity on null trials were distinct from those on high/low trials in multidimensional representational space. The fact that this was true for all epochs of the trial suggests that this separability was not simply a result of perceptual differences between memory samples.

Figure 8. .


Figure 8. 

Visualization of distances between conditions in multidimensional representational space. (A) Visualization of representational distances between high, low, and null conditions, in early visual cortex. The top panel shows the same MDS projection of data from the sample epoch from three perspectives: 3-D; a 2-D view of Dimension 1 versus Dimension 2; and a 2-D view of Dimension 1 versus Dimension 3. The middle panel shows three comparable views of the MDS projection of data from the late delay period, and the bottom panel shows three comparable views of the MDS projection of data from the response epoch. (B) MDS analyses of data from IPS, using the same display conventions as (A). Each marker represents one of the orientation bins (0–45°, 45–90°, 90–135°, 135–180°). Dark blue, light blue, and gray colors correspond to the high, low, and null conditions, respectively. a.u. = arbitrary units.

Secondary Analyses to Assess the Influence of the Previous Trial on Response-based IEMs

Recent perceptual history can bias behavior on the current trials (Fischer & Whitney, 2014), including during working memory tasks (Barbosa et al., 2020; Samaha, Switzky, & Postle, 2019), and it has been shown that the no-longer-relevant content of the previous trial can be decoded from EEG signals recorded during a visual working memory task (Bae & Luck, 2019). Consequently, we carried out a series of analyses to assess whether the response-related reconstructions from null trials (Figure 4), rather than reflecting internally generated stimulus representations, might instead be because of “spillover” of information processed during the previous trial. We tested this possibility with two approaches. First, we examined if the response of the previous trial could be reconstructed from patterns of activity of the current trial in the current data. In early visual cortex, we found that the response of the previous trial could indeed be reconstructed in all conditions, especially during the earlier portion of the trial, all ps < .031 (Figure 9). However, because above-baseline-level reconstruction of the previous-trial response was present at the very beginning of the trial (i.e., 0 sec), and reconstruction of the current-trial response did not emerge until 6 sec after trial onset, we believe it was unlikely that these two sets of results reflected the same piece of information. Furthermore, in IPS, reconstruction of the previous trial's response was almost absent, with the exception of three isolated time points across all three conditions (all ps < .03). This effect alone again cannot explain the sustained reconstructions of the response orientation on null trials.

Figure 9. .


Figure 9. 

Reconstructions of previous-trial response in early visual cortex and IPS in the high, low, and null conditions. (A) Time course of reconstructions of previous-trial response (indexed by the slope of reconstruction) in early visual cortex. (B) Time course of reconstructions of previous-trial response in IPS, from left to right: in high, low, and null condition. Red asterisks at the top of each plot denote significance of response reconstructions. Event labels below the x axis represent the Sample (S), Delay (D), and Response (R) periods, respectively. All p values were corrected with FDR across conditions and time points. Shaded areas denote ± 1 SEM. *p < .05.

A second approach to assess the possible influence of information from previous trials on response-related reconstructions from null trials was to redo the analyses after removing the trials for which the response was most similar to the response on the previous trial. We did this by first calculating the difference between each trial's response and the response on the previous trial, for all three conditions, and grouping the trials by difference values into nine 20°-wide bins. For high and null trials, the distribution of the differences was not uniform, χ2(8) = 19.5 and 81.9, p = .012 and p < .001, respectively (Figure 10), suggesting a potential influence from previous trials on the performance of the current trial. Next, for null trials, we removed the influence from the responses that were closest to the previous response (difference < 10°; bins highlighted in red in Figure 10) by excluding trials that belonged to this bin and repeating the IEM analyses on the remaining trials. Significant response reconstructions were still observed in this subset of null condition trials (Figure 11), increasing our confidence that the representation of response-related orientation information on null trials cannot be explained as simply reactivation of perceptual history from the previous trial.

Figure 10. .


Figure 10. 

Distribution of the distance between response on the current trial and that on the previous trial. The distribution of the distance between response on the current trial and that on the previous trial, in the high, low, and null conditions, indicated by the gray histograms (bin size = 20°). The red bar at the center indicates the bin with the smallest distance (≤ 10°).

Figure 11. .


Figure 11. 

Orientation reconstructions in early visual cortex and IPS in the high, low, and null conditions, after excluding highlighted trials in Figure 10. (A) Time course of orientation reconstructions (indexed by the slope of reconstruction) in early visual cortex, after excluding trials from the bin with the smallest distance in Figure 10, from left to right: in high, low, and null conditions. (B) Orientation reconstruction in the late delay period (8–10 sec after trial onset) in early visual cortex. (C) Time course of orientation reconstructions (indexed by the slope of reconstruction) in IPS, from left to right: in high, low, and null conditions. (D) Orientation reconstruction in the late delay period in IPS. Black and red lines correspond to sample and response, respectively. Black and red asterisks at the top of each plot denote significance of reconstructions, for sample and response, respectively. Green asterisks denote significance of difference between response and sample reconstructions. Event labels below the x axis represent the Sample (S), Delay (D), and Response (R) periods, respectively. All p values were corrected with FDR across conditions and time points. Shaded areas denote ± 1 SEM. *p < .05.

Finally, we examined whether the potency of the spillover effect varied with sample type, by sorting every high trial as a function of whether it was preceded by a high, low, or null trial. Results (not shown) indicated that the spillover effect was comparable for each trial type and that the time course of each mimicked the pattern seen in Figure 9.

DISCUSSION

The human brain processes massive amounts of information every day, from both external and internal sources. To explore how internally generated information is represented in the brain during working memory, we incorporated a null-sample condition into a delayed-recall task. First, we demonstrated that, after the presentation of a null sample, participants generated a neural representation that corresponded to the response that they would make at the end of the trial, confirming that our procedure was successful at producing internally generated working-memory representations. Next, we assessed cross-generalization of the neural representation of orientations between conditions and observed an asymmetric pattern in early visual cortex: IEMs trained on data from null trials generalized fully to data from visible-sample trials, but the converse was not true. This suggested some difference in the processing of internally generated representations versus conventional working memory representations that receive influences from both external and internal sources. This difference in neural codes was also evident when the data were projected into multidimensional representational space: The patterns of activity for high and low trials were clearly segregated from null trials in both early visual cortex and IPS. Therefore, stimulus information that is derived from an external source is represented differently than stimulus information that is generated internally.

Our findings might seem inconsistent with previous work that has demonstrated a shared neural code between visual working memory and visual imagery in early visual cortex (Albers et al., 2013). However, because visual imagery tasks often involve elements such as mental rotations (Albers et al., 2013) or retrocueing manipulations (Dijkstra et al., 2018), they typically refer overtly to previously presented (i.e., externally originated) information, and this may explain why similar neural codes are recruited by these two classes of task. It had thus remained unclear whether “purely” internally derived representations also share the same neural code as “conventional” working memory representations. The present results—indicating that the representation of orientation in early visual cortex fully generalizes from the null to the high and low conditions, but not in the other direction—suggest that all three conditions share the same purely internally generated neural codes and that conventional working memory representations contain one or more additional dimensions that are lacking from “purely” internally originated visual representations. The additional dimension(s) are likely related to processes that are involved in the initial processing of externally presented information.

The differences between working memory and internally originated imagery were also preserved in IPS, where we found that null and high/low trials did not generalize in either direction, although the effects were generally weaker compared with those in early visual cortex. These results are in line with previous work demonstrating failures to find evidence—in higher-order parietal and/or frontal cortex—for generalization of neural codes between working memory and visual perception (Rademaker et al., 2019), attention (Yu & Shim, 2019), and imagery (Albers et al., 2013).

What is the nature of the internally generated representations observed in the delay period of the null condition in the current study? One possibility is a preparatory motor code, similar to what has been demonstrated for visual working memory for orientation on a task that allowed for concurrent selection of visual and motor responses (van Ede, Chekroud, Stokes, & Nobre, 2019). If so, this would need to be a highly abstract code, akin to an intention, because the starting position of the probe in our experiment was randomized from trial to trial, and so participants would not have been able to plan their specific motor response prior to the onset of the response wheel. Another possibility is that they reflected internally generated representations of participants' best guess of the orientation of the sample. This would be consistent with the fact that the time course of the representation of orientation developed later in time in the null condition relative to the high and low conditions, especially in early visual cortex. Similarly, it has been observed that representations of visual imagery develop later in time than do representations associated with visual perception (Dijkstra et al., 2018) and with visual working memory (Albers et al., 2013). It is likely that internally generated representations are influenced by many stimulus-nonspecific factors, such as prior knowledge (Yu, Panichello, et al., 2020; Panichello et al., 2019; Bae et al., 2014, 2015) and recent history (Bae & Luck, 2019; Fischer & Whitney, 2014), and that these stimulus-nonspecific factors may serve, in part, as differentiating factors in the coding of externally driven versus internally generated information. Indeed, we did observe influences from the previous trial in the current experiment, although spillover from the previous trial alone cannot explain the sustained, robust representations of the response in the null condition. It should be possible to use the “null-sample” paradigm in combination with other visual tasks to better understand the nature of internally generated visual representations. For example, it would be interesting to compare internally generated representations directly with the codes that support visual perception. It would also be interesting to include confidence ratings in future tasks to better understand the subjective experience of the null condition. Finally, by combining the paradigm with ultra-high field fMRI, one would be able to investigate whether there exist layer-specific representations for purely internally generated representations.

Our results, together with previous work (Rademaker et al., 2019; Yu & Shim, 2019; Albers et al., 2013; Harrison & Tong, 2009), suggest a potential mechanism for how the brain processes information originating from different sources. Early visual cortex represents stimulus properties with a common neural code that is insensitive to behavioral/cognitive context, such that the same neural code is shared between visual perception, attention, and working memory, consistent with the sensorimotor recruitment hypothesis. However, early visual cortex also registers the source of origination of this information, such that externally originated and internally originated representations can be differentiated. This distinction between externally and internally originated representations was also observed in a higher-order cortical area, IPS, although perhaps with a slightly different pattern. These signals may underlie the neural basis for how the brain differentiates and maintains signals from different sources.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by NIH R01MH064498 to B. R. P.

Reprint requests should be sent to Qing Yu, Institute of Neuroscience, Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligence Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, 320 Yueyang Road, Shanghai, 200031, China, or via e-mail: qingyu@ion.ac.cn.

Author Contributions

Qing Yu: Conceptualization; Data curation; Formal analysis; Investigation; Methodology; Resources; Software; Validation; Visualization; Writing—Original draft; Writing—Review & editing. Bradley R. Postle: Conceptualization; Funding acquisition; Supervision; Writing—Original draft; Writing—Review & editing.

Funding Information

Bradley R. Postle, National Institute of Mental Health (http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/100000025), grant number: R01MH064498.

Diversity in Citation Practices

A retrospective analysis of the citations in every article published in this journal from 2010 to 2020 has revealed a persistent pattern of gender imbalance: Although the proportions of authorship teams (categorized by estimated gender identification of first author/last author) publishing in the Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience (JoCN) during this period were M(an)/M = .408, W(oman)/M = .335, M/W = .108, and W/W = .149, the comparable proportions for the articles that these authorship teams cited were M/M = .579, W/M = .243, M/W = .102, and W/W = .076 (Fulvio et al., JoCN, 33:1, pp. 3–7). Consequently, JoCN encourages all authors to consider gender balance explicitly when selecting which articles to cite and gives them the opportunity to report their article's gender citation balance. The authors of this article report its proportions of citations by gender category to be as follows: M/M = .794, W/M = .088, M/W = .118, and W/W = 0.

REFERENCES

  1. Albers, A. M., Kok, P., Toni, I., Dijkerman, H. C., & de Lange, F. P. (2013). Shared representations for working memory and mental imagery in early visual cortex. Current Biology, 23, 1427–1431. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.05.065, PMID:23871239 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Awh, E., & Jonides, J. (2001). Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 119–126. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01593-X, PMID:11239812 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 829–839. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1201, PMID:14523382 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bae, G.-Y., & Luck, S. J. (2019). Reactivation of previous experiences in a working memory task. Psychological Science, 30, 587–595. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619830398, PMID:30817224, PMCID:PMC6472175 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bae, G.-Y., Olkkonen, M., Allred, S. R., & Flombaum, J. I. (2015). Why some colors appear more memorable than others: A model combining categories and particulars in color working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 744–763. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000076, PMID:25985259 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bae, G.-Y., Olkkonen, M., Allred, S. R., Wilson, C., & Flombaum, J. I. (2014). Stimulus-specific variability in color working memory with delayed estimation. Journal of Vision, 14, 7. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1167/14.4.7, PMID:24715329 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Barbosa, J., Stein, H., Martinez, R. L., Galan-Gadea, A., Li, S., Dalmau, J., et al. (2020). Interplay between persistent activity and activity-silent dynamics in the prefrontal cortex underlies serial biases in working memory. Nature Neuroscience, 23, 1016–1024. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-020-0644-4, PMID:32572236, PMCID:PMC7392810 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B: Statistical Methodology, 57, 289–300. DOI: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bettencourt, K. C., & Xu, Y. (2016). Decoding the content of visual short-term memory under distraction in occipital and parietal areas. Nature Neuroscience, 19, 150–157. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4174, PMID:26595654, PMCID:PMC4696876 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433–436. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1163/156856897X00357, PMID:9176952 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cox, R. W. (1996). AFNI: Software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Computers and Biomedical Research, 29, 162–173. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014, PMID:8812068 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. D'Esposito, M., & Postle, B. R. (2015). The cognitive neuroscience of working memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 115–142. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015031, PMID:25251486, PMCID:PMC4374359 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dijkstra, N., Mostert, P., de Lange, F. P., Bosch, S., & van Gerven, M. A. J. (2018). Differential temporal dynamics during visual imagery and perception. eLife, 7, e33904. DOI:https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.33904, PMID:29807570, PMCID:PMC5973830 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309–331. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.128.3.309, PMID:10513398 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Ester, E. F., Sprague, T. C., & Serences, J. T. (2015). Parietal and frontal cortex encode stimulus-specific mnemonic representations during visual working memory. Neuron, 87, 893–905. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.07.013, PMID:26257053, PMCID:PMC4545683 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fischer, J., & Whitney, D. (2014). Serial dependence in visual perception. Nature Neuroscience, 17, 738–743. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3689, PMID:24686785, PMCID:PMC4012025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Foster, J. J., Bsales, E. M., Jaffe, R. J., & Awh, E. (2017). Alpha-band activity reveals spontaneous representations of spatial position in visual working memory. Current Biology, 27, 3216–3223. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.09.031, PMID:29033335, PMCID:PMC5661984 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Gilbert, C. D., & Li, W. (2013). Top–down influences on visual processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14, 350–363. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3476, PMID:23595013, PMCID:PMC3864796 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Harrison, S. A., & Tong, F. (2009). Decoding reveals the contents of visual working memory in early visual areas. Nature, 458, 632–635. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/nature07832, PMID:19225460, PMCID:PMC2709809 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Lawrence, S. J. D., van Mourik, T., Kok, P., Koopmans, P. J., Norris, D. G., & de Lange, F. P. (2018). Laminar organization of working memory signals in human visual cortex. Current Biology, 28, 3435–3440. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.08.043, PMID:30344121 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Liu, T., Cable, D., & Gardner, J. L. (2018). Inverted encoding models of human population response conflate noise and neural tuning width. Journal of Neuroscience, 38, 398–408. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2453-17.2017, PMID:29167406, PMCID:PMC5761616 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Love, J., Selker, R., Marsman, M., Jamil, T., Dropmann, D., Verhagen, J., et al. (2019). JASP: Graphical statistical software for common statistical designs. Journal of Statistical Software, 88, 1–17. DOI: 10.18637/jss.v088.i02 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  23. Luck, S. J., & Vogel, E. K. (2013). Visual working memory capacity: From psychophysics and neurobiology to individual differences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 391–400. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.006, PMID:23850263, PMCID:PMC3729738 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Mendoza-Halliday, D., Torres, S., & Martinez-Trujillo, J. C. (2014). Sharp emergence of feature-selective sustained activity along the dorsal visual pathway. Nature Neuroscience, 17, 1255–1262. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3785, PMID:25108910, PMCID:PMC4978542 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Muckli, L., & Petro, L. S. (2013). Network interactions: Non-geniculate input to V1. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23, 195–201. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.020, PMID:23402951 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Panichello, M. F., DePasquale, B., Pillow, J. W., & Buschman, T. J. (2019). Error-correcting dynamics in visual working memory. Nature Communications, 10, 3366. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-11298-3, PMID:31358740, PMCID:PMC6662698 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Pearson, J. (2019). The human imagination: The cognitive neuroscience of visual mental imagery. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 20, 624–634. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-019-0202-9, PMID:31384033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437–442. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1163/156856897X00366, PMID:9176953 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Rademaker, R. L., Chunharas, C., & Serences, J. T. (2019). Coexisting representations of sensory and mnemonic information in human visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 22, 1336–1344. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0428-x, PMID:31263205, PMCID:PMC6857532 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Riggall, A. C., & Postle, B. R. (2012). The relationship between working memory storage and elevated activity as measured with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 12990–12998. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1892-12.2012, PMID:22993416, PMCID:PMC3470886 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Roelfsema, P. R., & de Lange, F. P. (2016). Early visual cortex as a multiscale cognitive blackboard. Annual Review of Vision Science, 2, 131–151. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-vision-111815-114443, PMID:28532363 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Samaha, J., Sprague, T. C., & Postle, B. R. (2016). Decoding and reconstructing the focus of spatial attention from the topography of alpha-band oscillations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28, 1090–1097. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00955, PMID:27003790, PMCID:PMC5074376 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Samaha, J., Switzky, M., & Postle, B. R. (2019). Confidence boosts serial dependence in orientation estimation. Journal of Vision, 19, 25. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1167/19.4.25, PMID:31009526, PMCID:PMC6690400 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Serences, J. T. (2016). Neural mechanisms of information storage in visual short-term memory. Vision Research, 128, 53–67. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2016.09.010, PMID:27668990, PMCID:PMC5079778 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Serences, J. T., Ester, E. F., Vogel, E. K., & Awh, E. (2009). Stimulus-specific delay activity in human primary visual cortex. Psychological Science, 20, 207–214. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02276.x, PMID:19170936, PMCID:PMC2875116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Sprague, T. C., Adam, K. C. S., Foster, J. J., Rahmati, M., Sutterer, D. W., & Vo, V. A. (2018). Inverted encoding models assay population-level stimulus representations, not single-unit neural tuning. eNeuro, 5, ENEURO.0098-18.2018. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0098-18.2018, PMID:29876523, PMCID:PMC5987635 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. van Ede, F., Chekroud, S. R., Stokes, M. G., & Nobre, A. C. (2019). Concurrent visual and motor selection during visual working memory guided action. Nature Neuroscience, 22, 477–483. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0335-6, PMID:30718904, PMCID:PMC6420070 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. van Kerkoerle, T., Self, M. W., & Roelfsema, P. R. (2017). Layer-specificity in the effects of attention and working memory on activity in primary visual cortex. Nature Communications, 8, 13804. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms13804, PMID:28054544, PMCID:PMC5227065 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Wang, L., Mruczek, R. E. B., Arcaro, M. J., & Kastner, S. (2015). Probabilistic maps of visual topography in human cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 25, 3911–3931. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu277, PMID:25452571, PMCID:PMC4585523 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Watson, A. B., & Pelli, D. G. (1983). QUEST: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method. Perception & Psychophysics, 33, 113–120. DOI:https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03202828, PMID:6844102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Yu, Q., Panichello, M. F., Cai, Y., Postle, B. R., & Buschman, T. J. (2020). Delay-period activity in frontal, parietal, and occipital cortex tracks noise and biases in visual working memory. PLoS Biology, 18, e3000854. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000854, PMID:32898172, PMCID:PMC7500688 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Yu, Q., & Shim, W. M. (2017). Occipital, parietal, and frontal cortices selectively maintain task-relevant features of multi-feature objects in visual working memory. Neuroimage, 157, 97–107. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.05.055, PMID:28559190 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Yu, Q., & Shim, W. M. (2019). Temporal-order-based attentional priority modulates mnemonic representations in parietal and frontal cortices. Cerebral Cortex, 29, 3182–3192. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhy184, PMID:30124789 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Yu, Q., Teng, C., & Postle, B. R. (2020). Different states of priority recruit different neural representations in visual working memory. PLoS Biology, 18, e3000769. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000769, PMID:32598358, PMCID:PMC7351225 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of MIT Press

RESOURCES