Abstract
Movement is associated with power changes over sensory-motor areas in different frequency ranges, including beta (15–30 Hz). In fact, beta power starts decreasing before the movement onset (event-related desynchronization, ERD) and rebounds after its end (event-related synchronization, ERS). There is increasing evidence that beta modulation depth (measured as ERD-ERS difference) increases with practice in a planar reaching task, suggesting that this measure may reflect plasticity processes. In the present work, we analyzed beta ERD, ERS and modulation depth in healthy subjects to determine their changes over three regions of interest (ROIs): right and left sensorimotor and frontal areas, during a reaching task with the right arm. We found that ERD, ERS and modulation depth increased with practice with lower values over the right sensory-motor area. Timing of peak ERD and ERS were similar across ROIs, with ERS peak occurring earlier in later sets. Finally, we found that beta ERS of the frontal ROI involved higher beta range (23–29 Hz) than the sensory-motor ROIs (15–18 Hz). Altogether these results suggest that practice in a reaching task is associated with modification of beta power and timing. Additionally, beta ERS may have different functional meaning in the three ROIs, as suggested by the involvement of upper and lower beta bands in the frontal and sensorimotor ROIs, respectively.
I. Introduction
During movement planning, beta power (15–30 Hz) over the sensory-motor cortices starts decreasing and reaches a minimum at the end of the movement (ERD) followed by a rebound (ERS) [1]. ERD and ERS likely reflect the deactivation and subsequent reactivation of the somatosensory areas as a consequence of the increasing excitability of the motor cortex [2], [3]. Studies in animal and humans have demonstrated that beta power increases are related to a reduction of cortical excitability and increase of GABA levels [4], [5]. In a recent study with old healthy subjects and patients with Parkinson’s disease, we found that the difference between ERD and ERS, progressively increases over the sensory-motor areas, as well in a frontal region during 40 minutes of reaching movements. Also, beta modulation depth returned to baseline levels the day after, and correlated with retention indices of motor skills [6], [7]. A possible interpretation of this result is that repetitive movements triggered plasticity-related phenomena and the progressive increase of beta modulation may represent the gradual saturation of long-term potentiation phenomena. Here, we investigate whether practice-related beta power changes similarly occur in a population of healthy adults, with a focus on magnitude, timing and frequency of the peak ERD, ERS and beta modulation. Specifically, we focused on three regions: the left and right sensory-motor areas and a frontal area to assess whether different regions show movement-related differences in the beta electroencephalographic (EEG) activity.
II. Materials and Methods
A. Subjects
Twenty-six right-handed healthy subjects (age range: 20–68; mean ± SD: 40.8 ± 18.2, 16 female) were enrolled for this study. Experiments were conducted with the approval of our Institutional Review Board and each subject expressed written consent to participate to the study.
B. Experimental Design
Subjects performed 15 sets of 56 movements each in a 30-minute session. They were seated in front of a screen with their right upper-limb hidden by a surface and were asked to move a cursor with the right arm on a digitizing tablet to reach for targets appearing on the screen [8], [9]. An array of eight targets (1 cm diameter) equidistant (4 cm) from the center was displayed for entire session (Fig. 1a). Every 1.5s, one target randomly blackened for 400 ms and subjects had to reach it moving as soon, fast and accurately as possible, but without anticipation or guessing. Participants were also instructed to make overlapping out-and back trajectories without any correction or stop. Before the experiment, they were trained to reach a hit rate of at least 95%. High-density EEG (256-channels Electrical Geodesic Inc.) was recorded during the entire session, with the high-impedance amplifier Net Amp 300 and Net Station 4.3 (Electrical Geodesic Inc.). Sampling rate was 250 Hz, impedances were kept below 30 kΩ; the signal was referenced to the vertex Cz.
C. Data Analyses
For each movement we computed (Fig. 1): reaction time, time between target appearance and movement onset; peak velocity; hand path area, area included in the trajectory, normalized by squared path length; total movement time, between onset and end of the out and back movement. For each subject, movements exceeding two SD from the mean in any kinematic indices were excluded from further analysis.
EEG signal was bandpassed and Notch filtered (two-way least-square FIR filter between 1 and 80 Hz; Notch filter 60Hz), before being divided into 4-seconds epochs (−1 to 3 second around the stimulus onset). Data were visually inspected to remove artefactual channels and epochs. Additionally, we used Independent Component Analysis (ICA) with Principal Component Analysis to remove stereotypical artifacts such as muscle-related signal, blinks and saccades [10]. Finally, all the channels previously removed were replaced with spherical spline interpolation and data were averaged-referenced.
After the preprocessing, we centered each trial on the movement onset. Then, we computed Fast Fourier Transformation (Hanning taper, 20 ms time steps, 5 cycles adaptive window) in the beta range, i.e. between 15 and 30 Hz, over three regions of interests (ROIs): a left, right and frontal ROI, expressing consistent movement-related beta modulation across subjects [6], [7]. These ROIs were defined on the grand-average taking the electrode with the highest ERD, ERS, or beta modulation depth, and its six closest neighbors (Fig. 1d). For each ROI, we normalized the power of each trial with the average beta power of the session. Afterward, we computed ERD and ERS magnitude: ERD was defined as the minimum desynchronization occurring in a window between 200 ms before movement onset and 700 ms after it; ERS was the maximum value in a 1-s window after the movement (500 ms to 1500ms after movement onset). These windows were chosen in order to select the peak ERD and ERS (Fig. 1c). We also subtracted the ERD magnitude from the ERS to obtain the beta modulation depth (Fig. 1c). All the analysis and preprocessing of the data have been implemented in Matlab, using EEGLAB [11], Fieldtrip [12] toolboxes, as well as customized code.
D. Statistical Analyses
Changes in kinematic indices were assessed with repeated measure ANOVAs with sets as factors. For EEG measures we used mixed-model ANOVAs with ROIs and sets as within-subject factors. Violation of sphericity was corrected with Greenhouse-Geisser. Significant main effects were followed by Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparisons; time-frequency differences between ROIs were assessed through Nonparametric Permutation Testing with False Discovery Rate Correction. Correlation between kinematic and EEG data have been assess through Pearson’s coefficients with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. Results were considered significant for p-values <0.05. All statistical analyses were conducted with SPSS v25, Matlab 2017b and Fieldtrip toolbox for Matlab.
III. Results
A. Subjects reduce their total movement time with practice
All subjects completed the tasks without difficulty; movements had bell-shaped velocity profiles and were mainly straight. Kinematic results are summarized in Fig. 2. Briefly, we found that reaction time and peak velocity increased across sets, however they do not reach significance (F(4.86,121.40)=1.53, p=0.188; F(1.83,45.73)=1.29, p=0.285, respectively). Hand path area, an inverse index of trajectory accuracy, showed a borderline significant decrease across sets (F(6.47, 161.83)=1.96, p=0.069). Total movement time showed a significant effect of practice (F(3.24, 80.97)=8.01, p<0.001).
B. Beta modulation depth in the right sensory-motor area is lower compared to the other ROIs
We then analyzed ERD, ERS and beta modulation depth differences in the three ROIs, as well as their magnitude changes with practice. In agreement with previous results, we found that the right sensory-motor ROI, ipsilaterally to the movement, showed lower ERD, ERS and beta modulation depth than the other two ROIs (Fig. 3). Specifically, the right ROI ERD was significantly lower than the left (post hoc test p=0.006) and frontal (p=0.002) ones. Interestingly, the frontal ROI had borderline significant greater ERD than the left ROI (p=0.055). ERS and beta modulation depth were significantly lower in the right ROI (ERS: right vs. left p=0.009; right vs. frontal p=0.004; left vs frontal p=0.936 Modulation Depth: right vs. left p=0.006; right vs. frontal p=0.003; left vs. frontal p=0.743). Repeated measure ANOVAs also indicated a significant increase across sets for ERD, ERS and modulation depth (Tab. I); no interactions were found between ROIs and sets. We then correlated practice-related kinematic changes with ERD, ERS and beta modulation increases; however, we did not find any direct relationship between performance and beta modulation changes.
C. Timing of ERD and ERS is similar in the three ROIs
We investigated whether ERD and ERS timings at their peak occurrence were similar in the three ROIs (Fig. 4). We found no difference between ROIs (ERD: F(1.17, 29.16)=0.01 p=0.956; ERS: F(1.14, 28.60)=0.27, p=0.637); the results of the ANOVA also revealed that timing of peak ERS decreased in the course of practice (F(6.21, 155.15) = 3.74, p=0.001), while the ERD timing did not change (F(14,350)=1.22, p=0.258) and no interactions were found (ERD: F(11.99, 299.78)=0.70, p=0.751; ERS: F(11.57, 289.36)=0.74, p=0.703). Correlations between timings and kinematic parameters did not revealed any relationship between ERD and ERS time changes and performance.
D. ERS of the frontal ROI occurs in a different range of the beta band compared to the sensory-motor cortices
We finally verified whether the movement-related beta oscillatory changes occurred in the same frequency range in the three ROIs. Thus, we isolated the frequency of the peak ERD and ERS. As a first step, we checked whether ERD and ERS peak frequency changed across sets and ROIs. ANOVAs results on peak ERD did not reveal any effect of set (F(14,350)=0.47, p=0.946), ROI (F(2,50)=0.59, p=0.561) and interaction (F(28,700)=0.84, p=0.705). On the other hand, analysis on ERS peak showed a main effect of ROI (Fig. 5; F(2,50)= 7.93, p=0.001). Pairwise comparison revealed that the ERS frequency of the frontal ROI was higher than those of the sensory-motor ones (frontal vs left p=0.001; frontal vs right p=0.007) that did not differ from each other (p=1.000, Fig.4b). No effect of sets (F(7.1, 177.50)=1.28, p=0219) nor interaction were found (F(10.59, 264.81)=1.12, p=0.348). This difference of ERS frequency between the frontal and the sensory-motor regions was even more visible when we computed time-frequency representations of the three ROIs (Fig. 6) on all the trials. In fact, Nonparametric Permutation Testing with False Discovery Rate correction revealed that ERS occurred in the range between 15 and 18 Hz over the left sensory-motor ROI, while the frontal ROI ERS between 23 and 29 Hz (Fig. 7). We did not find any frequency difference between ROIs when we analyzed ERD, which mainly occurred around 19 Hz. Of note, we only compared frequency differences between the left and frontal ROIs, because the significant lower magnitude values of the right ROI could have biased the results. Finally, we did not find any correlation between practice-related peak frequency of both ERD and ERS and kinematic changes.
IV. DISCUSSION
In this study, we examined EEG oscillatory beta activity over the two sensory-motor areas and a frontal region, during upper limb reaching movements. We analyzed the magnitude of ERD, ERS and beta modulation depth. Moreover, we investigated whether timing and frequency of the peak ERD and ERS differed in the three ROIs and whether such parameters changed with practice. We found that ERD, ERS and beta modulation depth changed across sets in the three ROIs; however, the right ROI, ipsilateral to the movement, showed a lower modulation. Also, ERS peaks occurs earlier across sets. Finally, we found that the frontal ROI presented an ERS with constant peak in the upper beta range, while the two sensory-motor ROIs had ERS occurring in the lower beta range.
A. ERD and ERS magnitude changes with practice
During a 30-minute reaching task we found a progressive increase of ERD, ERS and beta modulation depth over all the ROIs. A possible explanation of such ERD, ERS increase might be fatigue: with time subjects may get tired and thus require a higher involvement of the sensory-motor cortex represented by a lower ERD and a higher ERS. However, behavioral data do not reveal any fatigue. Indeed, neither reaction time nor peak velocity decrease with practice. We thus speculated that the lack of correlation between practice- related magnitude changes and kinematic data can depend on the fact that beta modulation does not directly reflect movement characteristics, but instead it can be related to movement planning and execution of a movement through sensory-motor integration processes [13]–[15]. Also, practice-related beta modulation may represent plasticity and thus LTP-like phenomena in the sensorimotor cortex [5]–[7].
B. Beta modulation depth is reduced in the right sensory-motor area
In agreement with previous results [6], [7], we observed greater ERD, ERS and beta modulation over the sensory- motor cortex contralaterally to the moving arm compared to the ipsilateral ROI. In fact, this area has been reported to be active during voluntary movements [8], [16]. However, beta modulatory activity over the ipsilateral motor cortex showed, albeit to a lesser extent, a similar increase across sets, suggesting a direct involvement of such area in movement production. Previous works have speculated on the role of the ipsilateral sensory-motor cortex on movement planning and execution; its activation may be related to movement organization and mirror movement suppression [17]–[21]. Moreover, animal studies revealed that projections between proximal sensory-motor areas exist and thus the activation of the contralaterally motor areas could spread to the ipsilateral through callosal connections [22]–[24]. Also, ERS has been related to increased cortical inhibition in the contralateral but not in the ipsilateral motor cortex [25]–[27]. The different role of the motor cortices during voluntary movement may explain the magnitude differences of ERD, ERS and beta modulation. Finally, the involvement of the frontal cortex, which shows beta modulatory activity of similar magnitude to that of the contralateral motor cortex, could be explained by the different functions related to motor control that occur in the frontal area, such as attentional processes related to motor planning and performance as well as to the updating of the internal models that are necessary for efficient planning and execution [28].
C. ERS of the frontal ROI shifts toward higher frequencies within the beta band
One of the most innovative findings is that, compared to the sensory-motor areas, the frontal ERS is overall shifted toward the high beta range. Intriguingly, ERS peak occurred simultaneously over the three ROIs and does not present magnitude differences with the left ROI. Time-frequency representations revealed that the ERS in the left sensory-motor cortex occurs between 15 and 18 Hz, the frontal ROI shows an ERS in the higher beta range, between 23 and 29 Hz. The frontal cortex has several functions that require the integration of information from multiple regions of the brain. In fact, it is responsible for learning, memory, attention, and motivation, in order to plan the most appropriate behavior in response to internal and external stimuli. During a movement, the frontal cortex combines information from sensory and motor cortices; in particular, studies on reaching and catching movements underlined the importance of the frontal region during monitoring and correction of an ongoing performance and, most importantly, during updating of internal models [29]–[31]. Some studies hypothesized that beta activity originates from multiple sources and thus reflects different functional processes [16], [32]–[34]. Our result perfectly fits this idea that beta can have many roles that can be distinguished by differences in the predominant frequency. Also, our finding is further supported by previous studies showing that the ERS frequency following finger dorsal flexions is lower in the hand motor area, compared to the supplementary motor cortex [13], [14]. A possible explanation of this phenomenon may be that, after the movement ends, the sensory motor region and the frontal area show different oscillations that may be related to different processes.
V. Conclusion
These results provide novel information about the neurophysiological basis of motor control. Understanding brain activity of healthy participants is the base for the detection of biomarkers of cortical activity for neurological patients. Most importantly, knowing mechanisms underlying the acquisition and updating of internal models is crucial for selection of rehabilitation programs. Neuro-rehabilitation takes advantages of assumptions about how the brain controls movements; thus, it is important to know how motor control processes are organized and how these change as a consequence of a pathology [35]. One of the main problems in rehabilitation is the uncertainty about the best type of intervention or device in different pathologies [36]. In this context, theoretical studies are crucial to help therapists in defying whether is it possible to restore a motor pattern or if it is better to train patients to find adaptive strategies which can maximize the outcome [37]. Specifically, we found that different brain areas showing beta modulatory activity present differences in term of magnitude and frequency range. In this context, further studies are required to investigate the role of the frontal cortex in motor control, in physiological and pathological conditions and during rehabilitation.
TABLE I.
ERD | ERS | Modulation Depth | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
F | p | F | P | F | p | |
Sets | 4.77 | 0.001 | 13.89 | <0.001 | 13.95 | <0.001 |
ROI | 12.73 | 0.337 | 9.55 | <0.001 | 10.52 | <0.001 |
Sets*ROI | 0.60 | 0.762 | 0.70 | 0.679 | 0.80 | 0.594 |
Acknowledgment
Kinematic data were collected with MotorTaskManager software (E.T.T. Srl). We thank: A. Nelson, J. Lin, M. Bossini-Baroggi, for helping during data acquisition and preprocessing.
* This work was supported by NIH P01 NS083514 (MFG)
Contributor Information
Serena Ricci, CUNY School of Medicine, 10031, New York, NY, USA; Department of Informatics, Bioengineering, Robotics and System Engineering, University of Genova, 16145, Italy.
Elisa Tatti, CUNY School of Medicine, 10031, New York, NY, USA.
Ramtin Mehraram, Institute of Neuroscience, Newcastle University, NE17RU, UK.
Priya Panday, CUNY School of Medicine, 10031, New York, NY, USA.
M. Felice Ghilardi, CUNY School of Medicine, 10031, New York, NY, USA.
References
- [1].Pfurtscheller G. and Da Silva FHL, “Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles,” Clin. Neurophysiol, vol. 110, no. 11, pp. 1842–1857, 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [2].Rossi S. et al. , “Somatosensory processing during movement observation in humans,” Clin. Neurophysiol, vol. 113, no. 1, pp. 16–24, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [3].Cebolla AM and Cheron G, “Sensorimotor and cognitive involvement of the beta–gamma oscillation in the frontal N30 component of somatosensory evoked potentials,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 79, pp. 215–222, 2015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [4].McAllister CJ, Rönnqvist KC, Stanford IM, Woodhall GL, Furlong PL, and Hall SD, “Oscillatory beta activity mediates neuroplastic effects of motor cortex stimulation in humans,” J. Neurosci, vol. 33, no. 18, pp. 7919–7927, 2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [5].Hall SD, Barnes GR, Furlong PL, Seri S, and Hillebrand A, “Neuronal network pharmacodynamics of GABAergic modulation in the human cortex determined using pharmaco-magnetoencephalography,” Hum. Brain Mapp, vol. 31, no. 4, pp. 581–594, 2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [6].Nelson AB et al. , “Beta Oscillatory Changes and Retention of Motor Skills during Practice in Healthy Subjects and in Patients with Parkinson’s Disease,” Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol. 11. 2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [7].Moisello C. et al. , “Practice changes beta power at rest and its modulation during movement in healthy subjects but not in patients with Parkinson’s disease,” Brain and Behavior, vol. 5, no. 10. Hoboken, Oct-2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [8].Ghilardi M-F et al. , “Patterns of regional brain activation associated with different forms of motor learning,” Brain Res, vol. 871, no. 1, pp. 127–145, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [9].Ghilardi MF, Moisello C, Silvestri G, Ghez C, and Krakauer JW, “Learning of a sequential motor skill comprises explicit and implicit components that consolidate differently.,” J. Neurophysiol, vol. 101, no. 5, pp. 2218–2229, May 2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [10].Jung T-P et al. , “Removing electroencephalographic artifacts by blind source separation,” Psychophysiology, vol. 37, no. 2, pp. 163–178, Mar. 2003. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [11].Delorme A. and Makeig S, “EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis,” J. Neurosci. Methods, vol. 134, no. 1, pp. 9–21, Mar. 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [12].Oostenveld R, Fries P, Maris E, and Schoffelen J-M, “FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data.,” Comput. Intell. Neurosci, vol. 2011, p. 156869, 2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [13].Shimazu H. et al. , “Pre-movement gating of short-latency somatosensory evoked potentials,” Neuroreport, vol. 10, no. 12, pp. 2457–2460, 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [14].Cassim F. et al. , “Brief and sustained movements: differences in event-related (de) synchronization (ERD/ERS) patterns,” Clin. Neurophysiol, vol. 111, no. 11, pp. 2032–2039, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [15].Tatti E. et al. , “Beta modulation depth is not linked to movement features,” Front. Behav. Neurosci, vol. 13, p. 49, 2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [16].Salmelin R, Forss N, Knuutila J, and Hari R, “Bilateral activation of the human somatomotor cortex by distal hand movements,” Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol, vol. 95, no. 6, pp. 444–452, 1995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [17].Kristeva R, Cheyne D, and Deecke L, “Neuromagnetic fields accompanying unilateral and bilateral voluntary movements: topography and analysis of cortical sources,” Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. Potentials Sect, vol. 81, no. 4, pp. 284–298, 1991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [18].Taniguchi M. et al. , “Movement-related desynchronization of the cerebral cortex studied with spatially filtered magnetoencephalography,” Neuroimage, vol. 12, no. 3, pp. 298–306, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [19].Cheyne D, Bakhtazad L, and Gaetz W, “Spatiotemporal mapping of cortical activity accompanying voluntary movements using an event-related beamforming approach,” Hum. Brain Mapp, vol. 27, no. 3, pp. 213–229, 2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [20].Leocani L, Cohen LG, Wassermann EM, Ikoma K, and Hallett M, “Human corticospinal excitability evaluated with transcranial magnetic stimulation during different reaction time paradigms,” Brain, vol. 123, no. 6, pp. 1161–1173, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [21].Schaefer SY, Haaland KY, and Sainburg RL, “Hemispheric specialization and functional impact of ipsilesional deficits in movement coordination and accuracy.,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 47, no. 13, pp. 2953–2966, Nov. 2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [22].Pappas CL and Strick PL, “Anatomical demonstration of multiple representation in the forelimb region of the cat motor cortex.,” J. Comp. Neurol, vol. 200, no. 4, pp. 491–500, Aug. 1981. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [23].Jacobson S. and Trojanowski JQ, “The cells of origin of the corpus callosum in rat, cat and rhesus monkey.,” Brain Res, vol. 74, no. 1, pp. 149–155, Jul. 1974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [24].Pandya DN, Gold D, and Berger T, “Interhemispheric connections of the precentral motor cortex in the rhesus monkey.,” Brain Res, vol. 15, no. 2, pp. 594–596, Oct. 1969. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [25].Chen R, Yaseen Z, Cohen LG, and Hallett M, “Time course of corticospinal excitability in reaction time and self-paced movements,” Ann. Neurol, vol. 44, no. 3, pp. 317–325, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [26].Leocani L, Toro C, Zhuang P, Gerloff C, and Hallett M, “Event-related desynchronization in reaction time paradigms: a comparison with event-related potentials and corticospinal excitability,” Clin. Neurophysiol, vol. 112, no. 5, pp. 923–930, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [27].Rau C, Plewnia C, Hummel F, and Gerloff C, “Event-related desynchronization and excitability of the ipsilateral motor cortex during simple self-paced finger movements.,” Clin. Neurophysiol, vol. 114, no. 10, pp. 1819–1826, Oct. 2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [28].Jurkiewicz MT, Gaetz WC, Bostan AC, and Cheyne D, “Post-movement beta rebound is generated in motor cortex: evidence from neuromagnetic recordings,” Neuroimage, vol. 32, no. 3, pp. 1281–1289, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [29].Perfetti B. et al. , “Temporal Evolution of Oscillatory Activity Predicts Performance in a Choice-Reaction Time Reaching Task,” Journal of Neurophysiology, vol. 105, no. 1. Bethesda, MD, pp. 18–27, Jan-2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [30].Tombini M. et al. , “Brain activity preceding a 2D manual catching task,” Neuroimage, vol. 47, no. 4, pp. 1735–1746, 2009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [31].Contreras-Vidal JL and Kerick SE, “Independent component analysis of dynamic brain responses during visuomotor adaptation,” Neuroimage, vol. 21, no. 3, pp. 936–945, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [32].Pistohl T, Schulze-Bonhage A, Aertsen A, Mehring C, and Ball T, “Decoding natural grasp types from human ECoG,” Neuroimage, vol. 59, no. 1, pp. 248–260, 2012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [33].Cremoux S, Tallet J, Berton E, Dal Maso F, and Amarantini D, “Does the force level modulate the cortical activity during isometric contractions after a cervical spinal cord injury?,” Clin. Neurophysiol, vol. 124, no. 5, pp. 1005–1012, 2013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [34].Kilavik BE, Zaepffel M, Brovelli A, MacKay WA, and Riehle A, “The ups and downs of beta oscillations in sensorimotor cortex,” Exp. Neurol, vol. 245, pp. 15–26, 2013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [35].Horak FB, “Assumptions underlying motor control for neurologic rehabilitation,” in Contemporary management of motor control problems: Proceedings of the II STEP conference, 1991, pp. 11–28. [Google Scholar]
- [36].Piscitelli D, “Motor rehabilitation should be based on knowledge of motor control,” Arch. Physiother, vol. 6, no. 1, p. 5, 2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [37].Latash ML and Nicholas JJ, “Motor control research in rehabilitation medicine,” Disabil. Rehabil, vol. 18, no. 6, pp. 293–299, 1996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]